Artwork for podcast So Curious!
I Love This Song! Music Taste and the Brain
Episode 16th June 2023 • So Curious! • The Franklin Institute
00:00:00 00:37:57

Share Episode

Shownotes

So Curious is back! And to kick off this season on the science of music, we're looking at a question we've all asked before; why do we like the music we like?

To get to the bottom of this, Bey and Kirsten sit down with three experts across the fields of music cognition, psychology, and audio engineering to explore the ways our brains, personality, and experiences help shape musical preferences.

Links for today’s episode:

Transcripts

Speaker:

Hello, hello, hello, and welcome to season

Speaker:

four of So Curious, a podcast presented by the Franklin Institute, a science museum

Speaker:

in Philadelphia focused on celebrating human sciences and technology.

Speaker:

And this season, we are gassed up.

Speaker:

We're geeked, we're excited!

Speaker:

We're very excited, because we're going to

Speaker:

be exploring all the science and technology behind music, how our brains

Speaker:

understand music, how people interact with music, and so much more.

Speaker:

But before we get into all that, we should probably introduce ourselves.

Speaker:

So like I said, I'm Kirsten Michelle Cills.

Speaker:

I am Philadelphia's token terminally ill stand up comic.

Speaker:

I'm born and raised in the City of Brotherly Love.

Speaker:

I have cystic fibrosis, and because of

Speaker:

that, I work as an advocate for the Cystic Fibrosis Foundation, and I've also been

Speaker:

doing stand up comedy professionally for eight years.

Speaker:

And I split my time between telling jokes

Speaker:

and yelling, "Go Birds!" unprompted, any chance I get.

Speaker:

And I'm The Bul Bey!

Speaker:

I'm a Philadelphia based hip hop artist.

Speaker:

To prove it, here's a little clip.

Speaker:

"Yeah, I'm still out west.

Speaker:

Yeah, I'm about to get a check.

Speaker:

Yeah, I'm back at it again." I'm also a creative collaborator and a civic leader.

Speaker:

I do all things community.

Speaker:

I just love my city, and I love people,

Speaker:

and I love science, too, so I can't wait to get into this season.

Speaker:

All right, Bey, I already know you have a

Speaker:

musical background because you are a musician.

Speaker:

My musical background is not as strong as yours, but I did study

Speaker:

musical theater in college, so that's something.

Speaker:

Yeah, I was going to push back. You're the musician.

Speaker:

No, Bey. Come on.

Speaker:

No, you are!

Speaker:

You know what's so interesting, too, is I feel like I've had a very similar

Speaker:

experience where I was just kind of going through life and I didn't really take

Speaker:

stock fully of how much music was present, is present.

Speaker:

So no, I'm right there with you.

Speaker:

I didn't know music was ingrained in me.

Speaker:

So let's get into it.

Speaker:

This first episode of this season, season

Speaker:

four, we are going to be looking into the science behind music taste.

Speaker:

Like, why do we like the music that we like?

Speaker:

First, we'll be chatting with Dr.

Speaker:

Psyche Loui to learn why our brains like music.

Speaker:

Oooh! And then we're going to talk with psychology researcher Dr.

Speaker:

David Greenberg about how our personalities shape our music taste.

Speaker:

And to round out this inaugural episode of

Speaker:

season four, we'll be sitting down with audio engineer turned neuroscientist Dr.

Speaker:

Susan Rogers, about how elements of

Speaker:

recorded music impact how we personally respond to it.

Speaker:

All right. Well, then, Dr.

Speaker:

Psyche Loui, welcome to the So Curious podcast.

Speaker:

Can you tell us a little bit about who you are, what you do?

Speaker:

My name is Psyche Louie.

Speaker:

I am an associate professor of music and

Speaker:

creative practice at Northeastern University, where I direct a lab called

Speaker:

the MIND Lab, where mind stands for music, imaging, and neural dynamics.

Speaker:

So I do research in music and the brain, thinking about why we have music, how does

Speaker:

music tickle the brain, and how we can use that for greater good.

Speaker:

Tickle the brain, I like that!

Speaker:

I like that imagery.

Speaker:

And are you yourself a musician?

Speaker:

And if so, can we hear a little snippet?

Speaker:

Oh, my goodness!

Speaker:

Well, you're finding me on a fun day.

Speaker:

I do play musical instruments.

Speaker:

My main instrument is the violin, but I've recently picked up some mandolin, and we

Speaker:

were actually just rehearsing before I came here.(Mandolin playing ensues!)

Speaker:

Maybe that's all I got, but a little bit of mandolin, a little bit of violin.

Speaker:

I shouldn't spend the whole time

Speaker:

No, this is wonderful!

Speaker:

Because I think that's how I relax.

Speaker:

Why do our brains like music?

Speaker:

Is there some sort of dopamine response? Tell us about that.

Speaker:

Why does our brain like music?

Speaker:

Well, there are so many questions that surround that question.

Speaker:

So at the core of it, I think music plays with our expectations and our predictions.

Speaker:

So there are these long term expectations that we've learned and built up just by

Speaker:

virtue of being in our environments and being in the culture.

Speaker:

We also have kind of one-time predictions that are much more specific.

Speaker:

We might call those vertical expectations.

Speaker:

So, for example, if I told you I have a guitar pick in my pockets right now, you

Speaker:

might not have thought about that before, but now you know what I have in my pocket.

Speaker:

So that's a one time prediction, right?

Speaker:

So much of why we love music, I think, is

Speaker:

because music taps into these different kinds of predictions.

Speaker:

So, (more mandolin playing) that gives you

Speaker:

certain memories of maybe the last time you've heard that piece.

Speaker:

And now if I change that. (same mandolin example as before, but ending on a different note)

Speaker:

So that suddenly gives you a different set of expectations.

Speaker:

So much of why we love music is because

Speaker:

music taps into these different kinds of predictions.

Speaker:

I want to keep the conversation going and

Speaker:

ask you, why do we sometimes get chills when we hear music we particularly like?

Speaker:

What is that response?

Speaker:

I would imagine it's a pleasurable

Speaker:

response when people get the chills from a song.

Speaker:

Yeah, it's definitely a pleasurable

Speaker:

response, but it's also a little bit unexpected.

Speaker:

Chills are a grab bag of physiological descriptions that we have for those

Speaker:

musical moments that change our perception and our cognition.

Speaker:

We can record heart rate, for example, and

Speaker:

we can record breathing and skin conductance like how sweaty you are.

Speaker:

And we can see that if there are certain

Speaker:

pieces of music that gives you a chill, those specific moments that give you a

Speaker:

chill, you're actually sweatier and your heart is actually beating faster.

Speaker:

And that's been also tied to the dopaminergic response.

Speaker:

So earlier we're asking, oh, is it about dopamine?

Speaker:

And dopamine, of course, is a neurotransmitter.

Speaker:

That's a brain chemical that is really the currency of the reward system.

Speaker:

So we know that the central nervous

Speaker:

system - of not just humans, but many other animals - has evolved to want to

Speaker:

seek out food, and has evolved to seek out ways to procreate.

Speaker:

Right?

Speaker:

So that's a very kind of evolutionarily hardwired biological thing.

Speaker:

But then what humans can do over and above that is we're learning these associations

Speaker:

between these biological hardwired things and something that's a bit more abstract.

Speaker:

Right?

Speaker:

So if you'll go back to the old findings of Pavlov's dogs, where you have a dog

Speaker:

that's hearing a bell, and then given some meat powder.

Speaker:

And so meat powder, of course, makes the dog salivate.

Speaker:

And so over time, even if there's no meat

Speaker:

powder, if it hears just the bell, it will learn to salivate.

Speaker:

So there's this automatic conditioning that's going on.

Speaker:

And so, music seems to be somewhere between the bell and the meat powder.

Speaker:

So it's a series of learned associations

Speaker:

that might at some point be tied to these evolutionary advantages.

Speaker:

And so maybe part of that is about the way

Speaker:

music taps into movement, makes us want to move.

Speaker:

So by having moved together to music and to rhythm in music, over time, people

Speaker:

might have learned to associate music with social predictions.

Speaker:

And so then music is a way to bring us together.

Speaker:

And so at a core level, part of what, for

Speaker:

example, gives me chills is like when I'm hearing some piece of music really go

Speaker:

towards a goal and then it exceeds that goal.

Speaker:

So for example, in house music, we can

Speaker:

have a piece of music that's going faster and faster and faster and then suddenly it

Speaker:

just stops - and then there's a music happening again!

Speaker:

Right?

Speaker:

So that beat drop moment is very much tied to a surge of dopamine.

Speaker:

That's the dopamine rush that is in music.

Speaker:

And then in classical music, you also get these chord progressions.

Speaker:

You talk about like I, IV, V chord progressions, that's kind of very standard

Speaker:

chord progressions, but sometimes you might throw a different chord in there,

Speaker:

maybe there's like a flat sixth and suddenly it's like, ooohh, it's a little

Speaker:

bit unexpected, it's a little bit cooler than what you would expect.

Speaker:

So the unexpectedness, in music theory, it's called appoggiaturas.

Speaker:

And it turns out that a lot of folks, when

Speaker:

they hear those unexpected appoggiaturas, they report feeling like they want to cry.

Speaker:

So again, it's about playing into expectations.

Speaker:

Wow.

Speaker:

So I'm thinking about the fact that music is universal, of course, but are there

Speaker:

people who just do not get any enjoyment from listening to music?

Speaker:

Yeah, so specific musical anhedonia, that's the term that's been coined for

Speaker:

people who, you know, they're not generally depressed, right?

Speaker:

They love good food, they love a nice back

Speaker:

rub, they love long walks on the beach, but yet music just doesn't do it for them.

Speaker:

And we found a few cases like that and

Speaker:

that's been linked to differences in the way the auditory areas in the brain are

Speaker:

connected to the reward areas in the brain.

Speaker:

So given that the reward areas that are

Speaker:

rich in dopamine are how we're learning from the acoustic signal of music to form

Speaker:

these expectations and to motivate all sorts of behavior.

Speaker:

It makes sense that the auditory areas being less connected to the reward centers

Speaker:

are what give rise to the lack of music rewards.

Speaker:

So let me ask about genres, like pop.

Speaker:

Some people say that pop is easier for people to get into than, let's say,

Speaker:

freeform jazz or traditional music in other cultures.

Speaker:

Do things like exposure and family affect

Speaker:

how we respond to different types of music?

Speaker:

Yeah, for sure.

Speaker:

I would say that your past experiences

Speaker:

play a huge role in how you respond to music.

Speaker:

So if you grew up in a classical

Speaker:

background, chances are you've had more exposure to that in the past.

Speaker:

And then, of course, this interacts nonlinearly with age.

Speaker:

So we also know that the whole reward system is developing as we grow up.

Speaker:

There are certain time windows where there

Speaker:

are sensitive periods of development of our reward system.

Speaker:

So if you look at someone who's lived for, let's say, 70 years and you ask them, hey,

Speaker:

what are the pieces of music that you love the most?

Speaker:

Chances are they'll mention music from their adolescence.

Speaker:

So there's this golden age where this

Speaker:

reminiscence bump, when you are especially receptive.

Speaker:

Music that you find in that age range, you

Speaker:

find to be especially rewarding and memorable to listen to.

Speaker:

What topics are you researching now that you're most excited about?

Speaker:

Well, I like to find out what creativity is and how we can measure that.

Speaker:

So we have a new musical scale that's based on mathematical ratios that are

Speaker:

different from all existing scales in the world, and we're trying to come up with a

Speaker:

way to let people jam and let people compose tunes in that way.

Speaker:

And then we also want to record brain

Speaker:

potentials while people are listening to creative tunes in this new musical system

Speaker:

and use that as a way to find out how the brain enables creativity.

Speaker:

And we're also, on the other hand, looking

Speaker:

at nursing home - going to different nursing homes and designing music based

Speaker:

interventions that are a little bit novel ways to get at brain rhythms that might be

Speaker:

disrupted in people with Alzheimer's disease.

Speaker:

So, yeah, so two kind of arms of the work, some that's more kind of exploratory and

Speaker:

some that's more novel interventions for people who need it the most.

Speaker:

Yeah, that's incredible! Woah

Speaker:

I know right! I know.

Speaker:

Well, this is all so much to look forward to.

Speaker:

You're doing such great work in what you do with MIND Lab and with Northeastern.

Speaker:

And thank you so much for taking the time to chat with Bey and I.

Speaker:

Thank you for having me.

Speaker:

Great to talk to you!

Speaker:

All right. Thank you so much.

Speaker:

Dr. Louis.

Speaker:

We asked to hear a sample, but we

Speaker:

definitely were not expecting a live performance.

Speaker:

Of course. That's what this is all about.

Speaker:

No, we were not!

Speaker:

But now that we understand why our brains

Speaker:

like music in general, I say let's learn about how our

Speaker:

personalities shape what specific music we listen to.

Speaker:

And Dr. Greenberg is the man for that.

Speaker:

So, Dr.

Speaker:

Greenberg, thank you so much for joining us on the So Curious podcast.

Speaker:

How are you? I'm well.

Speaker:

Thanks for having me on.

Speaker:

Please introduce yourself and what you do.

Speaker:

I'm a psychologist and a neuroscientist

Speaker:

and I study the impact of music on the brain and society.

Speaker:

And are you yourself a musician? Could we hear a clip?

Speaker:

I consider myself first and foremost a musician.

Speaker:

That's how I got to wanting to understand

Speaker:

how music impacts the brain and impacts the mind and impacts people.

Speaker:

I grew up mainly studying and learning jazz improvisation.

Speaker:

John Coltrane was my main inspiration and hero.

Speaker:

There's some clips of a concert I did a couple of years ago specifically for

Speaker:

Coltrane, who spent some years in Philadelphia.

Speaker:

Yes, he did.

Speaker:

Nice. (David's performance plays)

Speaker:

We want to talk about your research.

Speaker:

A lot of your work deals with what are called the Big Five personality traits.

Speaker:

Can you explain what those Big Five are?

Speaker:

The Big Five personality traits is the

Speaker:

most contemporary and up to date model of how we understand who we are.

Speaker:

So whenever we're dealing with the topic

Speaker:

of personality we're really asking the question of who are we?

Speaker:

What motivates us?

Speaker:

The Big Five, it understands personality in terms of five dimensions.

Speaker:

So the five traits are openness to experience, conscientiousness,

Speaker:

extroversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism.

Speaker:

And how does someone's Big Five score affect their musical taste?

Speaker:

So I was a senior scientific consultant for Spotify and an advisor there for about

Speaker:

four years and I was helping lead a team to look at how people's, Spotify playlists

Speaker:

and histories were able to predict their Big Five personality traits.

Speaker:

And we found that overall we could

Speaker:

actually make interesting predictions about people's traits.

Speaker:

For example, people who are open to

Speaker:

experience have very wide ranging preferences for a variety of genres.

Speaker:

Beyond that, the most recent publication

Speaker:

that we've had, we've looked at hundreds of thousands of people across 50 different

Speaker:

countries and we find that actually the links between personality traits and

Speaker:

musical preferences are consistent across countries.

Speaker:

So an extrovert in Thailand or in India is

Speaker:

preferring music that is energetic and rhythmic as well as people in Brazil who

Speaker:

will also prefer music that is also energetic and rhythmic even though they

Speaker:

don't speak the same language and so forth.

Speaker:

So we're seeing that across countries

Speaker:

people are using music as a way to express their own internal traits.

Speaker:

And can I ask, like, when you saw the results, were you surprised?

Speaker:

There's been about 20 years of research in this area before we conducted this study -

Speaker:

which it's definitely one of, it might be the largest on the topic to date.

Speaker:

And so we had a lot of different

Speaker:

hypotheses that we had to go on and certain things made sense.

Speaker:

For example, people who are higher in openness to experience are going to prefer

Speaker:

more jazz and classical music and music that has more complexity to it.

Speaker:

People who are agreeable are going to tend

Speaker:

to like more mellow, sensual, romantic music.

Speaker:

But one of the things that was surprising was that people who score high on the

Speaker:

trait neuroticism, which is people who have a tendency towards stress and

Speaker:

anxiety, preferred music that was more intense.

Speaker:

So music from heavy metal genres and punk and so forth.

Speaker:

And now that's on average.

Speaker:

Why that was surprising was because we

Speaker:

thought that it could also go the other way, meaning that people who

Speaker:

have a certain degree of stress would then be seeking and preferring music that might

Speaker:

be more calming, like mellow music, in order to decrease their stress.

Speaker:

Whereas on average, what we are finding is that people are seeking music that

Speaker:

reflects the stress and also potentially even reinforces it.

Speaker:

So that's an interesting area that was a

Speaker:

surprise and an area that we want to be researching more, because neuroticism is a

Speaker:

predictor of many different types of mental health conditions.

Speaker:

And the more we understand it, the better

Speaker:

we can then use music to treat those conditions.

Speaker:

And there are likely subsets of people who score higher neuroticism that may use

Speaker:

music, for example, as catharsis in different ways.

Speaker:

And so we have to really understand a larger picture of it because, again, at

Speaker:

the end of the day, the Big Five is more of the starting point of personality.

Speaker:

It's the basic tendencies that we have, but it's not the end of the story.

Speaker:

So it's really, for us in that respect, a starting point.

Speaker:

Yeah.

Speaker:

And I wanted to ask you, David, what kind of music do you like?

Speaker:

I mean, I know that's probably a reductive

Speaker:

question because you work in this field, but...

Speaker:

It's a great question.

Speaker:

I sing to my daughter, so that's a lot of

Speaker:

the music that goes on in my life these days.

Speaker:

But I grew up with jazz.

Speaker:

When I learned how to improvise as a

Speaker:

musician, that was what I would say, like an opening or a new access point in terms

Speaker:

of just consciousness to play spontaneously with other people.

Speaker:

And that really then grew into discovering music in a variety of other areas.

Speaker:

And I study a lot of, now, what I would

Speaker:

consider music that has a spiritual undertone to it.

Speaker:

When we look at the music of John Coltrane, for example, his music not only

Speaker:

was about political and social issues, but also about spirituality.

Speaker:

So he had a deep religious experience.

Speaker:

And so when I first began to study

Speaker:

Coltrane's music, that was the beginning of understanding and trying to explore

Speaker:

different aspects of spirituality and music.

Speaker:

And so that's from a variety of different

Speaker:

religions and faiths and so forth, and how music evokes awe, how music evokes wonder.

Speaker:

And that then crosses over with the

Speaker:

science part of me, as well as the musician.

Speaker:

Awesome.

Speaker:

And circling back to improvisational jazz, how does that correlate to the Big Five?

Speaker:

What direct lines can you draw there?

Speaker:

And I want to know the same about religious music, like you said.

Speaker:

Right. So I'll start with the last one.

Speaker:

Preferences for religious music in the US.

Speaker:

Are very interesting, particularly because of the Bible Belt and so forth.

Speaker:

So in the South, you have people scoring high on religiosity.

Speaker:

But then also there's such a connection with country music in those areas.

Speaker:

So you actually see this correlation.

Speaker:

But then it's a prime example of correlation doesn't imply causation, so we

Speaker:

don't necessarily know that that's the true relationship.

Speaker:

We still need to unpack that more.

Speaker:

But in general, then, when we look at

Speaker:

spirituality, which is different from religiosity or religious affiliation, that

Speaker:

has strong links to openness to experience.

Speaker:

And so people who score high on openness

Speaker:

tend to also seek different spiritual aspects within music.

Speaker:

People who are also high on trait spirituality then will tend to listen to

Speaker:

music that is outside the box, can be more avantgarde.

Speaker:

And interestingly, just in some of our own research not yet published, we will

Speaker:

publish it soon, that music seems to provide a greater window into people's

Speaker:

trait of spirituality than the Big Five personality traits does.

Speaker:

In fact, if someone is reacting to music,

Speaker:

if you both provided your playlist, then that would give me a better sense of your

Speaker:

spirituality and it would give machine learning algorithms a better sense of your

Speaker:

spirituality than filling out a personality trait inventory.

Speaker:

Wow, that's interesting.

Speaker:

We have to circle back and send you some playlists!

Speaker:

Yeah right!

Speaker:

The jazz part of it is also openess to experience.

Speaker:

Yeah, I was going to say I imagine it

Speaker:

would be right openness with improvisational...

Speaker:

Right, but it goes beyond genre.

Speaker:

Our research, basically we stopped ten years ago looking at people as genres that

Speaker:

they like and more, looked at the underlying qualities of the attributes.

Speaker:

Because within jazz alone you have avantgarde, you have Sun Ra and so forth.

Speaker:

And then on the other end you have Kenny G or Chris Botti and smooth jazz.

Speaker:

So people who like different types of jazz music have different personality traits.

Speaker:

Those who like the really avant garde are

Speaker:

really open, also can tend to be extroverted.

Speaker:

Those who like more the smooth jazz on the

Speaker:

other extreme, for example, are typically more agreeable and more conscientious.

Speaker:

What other factors around personality have

Speaker:

been found to affect music taste, kind of beyond the framework of the Big Five?

Speaker:

There's other models of personality that we've looked at, for example, what's

Speaker:

called the empathizing or systemizing framework of understanding.

Speaker:

So if someone's very high on systemizing, they tend to prefer music that has more

Speaker:

intense, complex and cerebral aspects and features to the music.

Speaker:

And then that actually leads us to understanding autism, because people who

Speaker:

are autistic tend to score high on systemizing.

Speaker:

And so, in some of our initial research we

Speaker:

see the connections between musical preferences and systemizing and autism.

Speaker:

And then there's other aspects which are

Speaker:

interesting in terms of what I mentioned before, mental health.

Speaker:

So, when we look at personality, like I

Speaker:

mentioned before, big Five is just a starting point.

Speaker:

But what defines us as who we are is also

Speaker:

our narratives, our stories, how we adapt to certain situations.

Speaker:

And so when we look at the themes that people are drawn to in music, for example,

Speaker:

we see things like lyrics that communicate being tentative or not, that those have

Speaker:

certain associations with different types of mental health conditions and same thing

Speaker:

about body image and violence and so forth.

Speaker:

So the themes in music also can point us to different qualities in the person that

Speaker:

are beyond just about the Big Five basic tendencies.

Speaker:

Are the listeners aware of their own taste, of the patterns that they have in

Speaker:

terms of choosing songs or what they might be leaning towards?

Speaker:

I think people have a certain understanding intuitively.

Speaker:

I think that they know what works for them and what doesn't.

Speaker:

Sometimes it could take people a long time to find that song.

Speaker:

If someone just had a breakup and they

Speaker:

really need something that's cathartic, it could take days, if not months, to really

Speaker:

find that one song at the right time, at the right moment that then opens up that

Speaker:

surge of emotion that they need in terms of healing or a cathartic experience.

Speaker:

And so I think that with technology there's ways now to speed that up.

Speaker:

Underneath it, there are certain things

Speaker:

that any individual, any listener just can't have or understand.

Speaker:

For example, you take one song and you

Speaker:

might describe it in five or ten characteristics, but really there's

Speaker:

hundreds and thousands of different configurations of song patterns.

Speaker:

So once you grasp that, that's where then, like AI and machine learning is really

Speaker:

going to be advancing us in the next decade to then be able to show us a new

Speaker:

way that we can understand and use music to help us and to help each other.

Speaker:

Amazing.

Speaker:

So when you meet someone you've never

Speaker:

spoken to before, right then, and they tell you their favorite song, are you able

Speaker:

to infer what you think their personality might be like?

Speaker:

Yeah.

Speaker:

So there's of course outliers, but there's an incredible amount that if someone tells

Speaker:

you about the music that they like, if it's just one song or a group of songs,

Speaker:

you can infer certain aspects about their personality traits.

Speaker:

I'll end on an example, which is that I went for a music lesson.

Speaker:

I've been trying to work on voice.

Speaker:

My initial instrument is saxophone.

Speaker:

And the first thing we did in the lesson

Speaker:

is we just freely improvised for the first maybe 20 minutes.

Speaker:

And I never really spoken with him before,

Speaker:

so we didn't know each other personally, but I knew of him.

Speaker:

And then the first words he said to me were, "Wow, you've lived an interesting

Speaker:

but long life", which was a very interesting comment.

Speaker:

But what he was communicating to me was

Speaker:

like, "Hey, I kind of like, see where you're at, where you've been, and I can

Speaker:

see what you've been expressing through the music" because of the emotional

Speaker:

aspects of it and the psychological aspects that I was signaling either

Speaker:

consciously or unconsciously through the music.

Speaker:

What did you say back? Wow.

Speaker:

Yeah. What did you say back?

Speaker:

Aww shucks!

Speaker:

Yeah, I don't remember what I said back, but I

Speaker:

think it was just kind of like an understanding.

Speaker:

Yeah, wow! Beautiful.

Speaker:

Thank you, Dr.

Speaker:

Greenberg, for coming on to hang out with us.

Speaker:

And now to close out, we are joined by Dr.

Speaker:

Susan Rogers to share with us more about how music taste is formed.

Speaker:

Could you introduce yourself and what it is you do?

Speaker:

Well, I'm a semi-retired professor.

Speaker:

In my former life, years ago, I was a

Speaker:

record maker, an engineer, and a record producer.

Speaker:

And then I left to go to academia, earn a PhD in music perception and cognition.

Speaker:

And for the last 15 years, I've been teaching both record production and

Speaker:

psychoacoustics at Berklee College of Music in Boston.

Speaker:

Oooh, Psychoacoustics?

Speaker:

I can't wait to get into that!

Speaker:

But you have done some amazing things.

Speaker:

Can you talk about some of those amazing things a little bit?

Speaker:

Yeah, I started my career in Southern

Speaker:

California, where I was born and raised, and I started in 1978.

Speaker:

There weren't that many women - and there aren't now - as engineers.

Speaker:

You wouldn't see many women record producers.

Speaker:

I didn't know of any.

Speaker:

But I had more modest ambitions.

Speaker:

I just wanted to be in the game.

Speaker:

So I started my career as an audio tech.

Speaker:

What an audio tech would do, and still

Speaker:

does, is repair the equipment, the tape machines and the console.

Speaker:

And I was working as a technician in Hollywood when my favorite artist in the

Speaker:

world, who happened to be Prince, put out the word in the grapevine that he was

Speaker:

looking for a technician to come and join him full time in Minnesota.

Speaker:

So I joined him and did Purple Rain,

Speaker:

Around the World In A Day, the Parade album, Sign of the Times, and then all the

Speaker:

other stuff we did during that period, which was Sheila E.

Speaker:

and the Time and Vanity Six and all that stuff.

Speaker:

When I finished with him, we had a good run,

Speaker:

Yeah you did! But it was very very intense.

Speaker:

Yeah, seriously!

Speaker:

It was very very intense.

Speaker:

So we parted ways, amicably, and then I

Speaker:

began my career as an independent engineer and mixer and record producer.

Speaker:

Had big success in the late nine ties with Bare Naked Ladies.

Speaker:

By big success, I mean a hit record as a record producer.

Speaker:

And that's back in the days, and it's still the days today, but

Speaker:

you'd get a big royalty check if you had points on a record as producers did.

Speaker:

So you'd get that big royalty check, and

Speaker:

then six months later, you'd get another one.

Speaker:

Those were the glory days.

Speaker:

But with that money, I was able to leave the music business and enter academia and

Speaker:

pursue the sciences, which I love tremendously, and get my PhD.

Speaker:

This is all so amazing, but we brought you

Speaker:

on specifically because we want to talk about your work around music taste.

Speaker:

So, you wrote a book called This Is What

It Sounds Like:

What the Music You Love Says About You.

It Sounds Like:

I would just love to hear more about this from you.

It Sounds Like:

Yeah, these are all based on things that I

It Sounds Like:

learned in grad school, studying music, perception and cognition.

It Sounds Like:

And I threw in there, for good measure,

It Sounds Like:

something that we knew about in the recording studio.

It Sounds Like:

When we listen to a given record, there are at least seven different dimensions on

It Sounds Like:

that record that can, independently of the others, give us a little treat.

It Sounds Like:

And by treat, I mean a little release of dopamine.

It Sounds Like:

So let's imagine that you're listening to a record for the very first time.

It Sounds Like:

You've never heard it before, someone's playing it for you.

It Sounds Like:

And what your brain is going to do is perform a scan of these seven dimensions.

It Sounds Like:

And what you're seeking is your

It Sounds Like:

personalized treats, a match between that dimension and your listener profile.

It Sounds Like:

Now, all you need to do when you're scanning that record is find one treat.

It Sounds Like:

I mean, it's great if you can find two or three, but if you find one, that can be

It Sounds Like:

enough to make you go, yeah, I'm into it, I like it. It's good.

It Sounds Like:

I love it.

It Sounds Like:

I think we want to just go a little bit more into these dimensions that you talk

It Sounds Like:

about, these seven ones, like how they are practically applied, like how

It Sounds Like:

different people respond to them, and how do we build our own listener profiles.

It Sounds Like:

Yes! So, I love the saying from the biologist D'Arcy Thompson.

It Sounds Like:

He said, "Everything is the way it is because it got that way." So starting from

It Sounds Like:

even before you're born, your listener profile is starting to form.

It Sounds Like:

Because when you're floating around in

It Sounds Like:

there in the womb, you can hear Mom's voice.

It Sounds Like:

In the third trimester, you can hear Dad's voice too.

It Sounds Like:

So you're hearing the melodies of speech in your environment.

It Sounds Like:

After you get born, you begin learning - here's how they use their voices when

It Sounds Like:

they're trying to warn me, don't touch that.

It Sounds Like:

When they're trying to reprimand me, I told you not to touch that!

It Sounds Like:

When they're trying to calm me down, when they're trying to amp me up and get me to

It Sounds Like:

wake up, they use their voices differently.

It Sounds Like:

Different dynamics, different melodies.

It Sounds Like:

So we begin learning the emotions that underlie melody and

It Sounds Like:

loudness changes and tempo changes and things like that.

It Sounds Like:

So your listener profile is forming throughout your life.

It Sounds Like:

It's getting stronger every time you have a positive musical experience.

It Sounds Like:

So of these seven dimensions that I'm describing in the book, four of them are

It Sounds Like:

musical, and three of them are aesthetic , and they are dimensions that apply to

It Sounds Like:

film and books and television and dance and all art forms.

It Sounds Like:

So of the musical dimensions, it's the familiar ones.

It Sounds Like:

It's rhythm, lyrics, melody and timbre.

It Sounds Like:

Timbre refers to sound itself.

It Sounds Like:

You can take a given song and you can notate it on a score.

It Sounds Like:

Here's what its rhythm is, here's what its tempo is, its time signature.

It Sounds Like:

Here's its melody, here's its lyrics.

It Sounds Like:

But you could play that record on a great variety of different instruments.

It Sounds Like:

We tend to have preferences for guitar sounds and drum sounds and

It Sounds Like:

keyboard sounds and we have our preferences for the timbres that we like.

It Sounds Like:

Rhythm is associated with how your body likes to move.

It Sounds Like:

So I'd like you guys to think about being at a party.

It Sounds Like:

A variety of music is playing, let's say

It Sounds Like:

let's say it's some intergenerational thing like a wedding.

It Sounds Like:

So there's people of different ages and

It Sounds Like:

generations there, so they're going to play a variety of music.

It Sounds Like:

What song or record would they play that

It Sounds Like:

would get your body out on that dance floor?

It Sounds Like:

And when they did, how would you move?

It Sounds Like:

Would you do that rock thing where you go up and down like on a pogo stick?

It Sounds Like:

Or if it's maybe you like a Latin groove

It Sounds Like:

and you like something with more syncopation and you'll go side to side

It Sounds Like:

where your shoulders go one direction, your hips go the other.

It Sounds Like:

So I'd be out on that floor if I heard a

It Sounds Like:

groove that facilitated the dance I like to do.

It Sounds Like:

Rhythm is associated with our bodies.

It Sounds Like:

Melody is associated with the emotions

It Sounds Like:

that we pick up on from singing and from melodic instruments.

It Sounds Like:

Some listeners are deeply into it.

It Sounds Like:

So much so that they can get full

It Sounds Like:

enjoyment from records that are sung in a language that they don't speak.

It Sounds Like:

So a lot of people say, "Oh, I never

It Sounds Like:

listen to the words." Believe them when they say that!

It Sounds Like:

The lyrics are processed over here, left temporal lobe.

It Sounds Like:

For most of us, where we process speech.

It Sounds Like:

Melody's on the right hand side.

It Sounds Like:

So many of us will have deep passion for records sung in a language we don't speak.

It Sounds Like:

No, that's so true.

It Sounds Like:

That's so awesome.

It Sounds Like:

Yes, definitely true of me.

It Sounds Like:

And what I'm responding to is the passion in the vocals and in the melody, the

It Sounds Like:

intonation of the vocal performance, not the information or the words.

It Sounds Like:

Those are the four musical dimensions, then our aesthetic dimensions.

It Sounds Like:

Authenticity refers to where you perceive those performances to be coming from.

It Sounds Like:

Our preferences on that dimension might be for technical perfection.

It Sounds Like:

You might want to be listening to - if

It Sounds Like:

we're going to focus in on vocalists - you might want to listen to an Ella Fitzgerald

It Sounds Like:

or a Frank Sinatra, someone who was technically perfect.

It Sounds Like:

Or you may like your performance gestures

It Sounds Like:

to come from the heart or from the belly button or from the hips.

It Sounds Like:

For me, I love my Ella Fitzgerald, but boy, do I love Howlin' Wolf.

It Sounds Like:

Boy, do I love that blues music that just

It Sounds Like:

comes from the gut and from the belly button.

It Sounds Like:

I listen for that.

It Sounds Like:

I'll take a bad performance from the gut

It Sounds Like:

over a technically perfect but timid performance from the neck up.

It Sounds Like:

So that's authenticity.

It Sounds Like:

Realism versus abstraction has something

It Sounds Like:

to do with the kind of visualization that you most enjoy when you're listening to

It Sounds Like:

your favorite music. When you're listening to your favorite music, the kind of music

It Sounds Like:

that you love the most, what do you see in your mind's eye?

It Sounds Like:

Now, some people don't see anything.

It Sounds Like:

Some people see abstract shapes and colors.

It Sounds Like:

Some people see nature scenes, the beach or the mountains or a meadow or something.

It Sounds Like:

The most common visualization is you see scenes from your life, the people and

It Sounds Like:

places and events that were important to your life, when you hear this music.

It Sounds Like:

For other people, they visualize a story from the lyrics.

It Sounds Like:

Many of us picture ourselves playing or singing, and many of us picture the

It Sounds Like:

artist, either maybe in concert or in my case, in the recording studio.

It Sounds Like:

The reason it affects our sweet spot on the dimension of realism versus

It Sounds Like:

abstraction is some records lend themselves to the kind of visualization

It Sounds Like:

that you like best and other records don't.

It Sounds Like:

Then the very last dimension is one that's

It Sounds Like:

the easiest one of all to explain - novelty versus familiarity.

It Sounds Like:

Some of us, and I'm one of them, prefer a higher degree of innovation and

It Sounds Like:

experimentation than what you hear on the pop charts.

It Sounds Like:

Other folks like music in classic styles,

It Sounds Like:

your classic reggae or disco or bebop jazz or Bruce Springsteen - classic rock.

It Sounds Like:

Because when they select a new record, they want that technical perfection.

It Sounds Like:

They're not listening for innovation.

It Sounds Like:

We've all got sweet spots on these seven dimensions, and that's collectively our

It Sounds Like:

listener profile, which is unique to all of us.

It Sounds Like:

Well, thank you so much.

It Sounds Like:

We really appreciate it. Dr.

It Sounds Like:

Susan Rogers this was such a pleasure. Awesome.

It Sounds Like:

Thank you so much. Loved talking with you.

It Sounds Like:

Thank you, bye!

It Sounds Like:

Thank you. Dr.

It Sounds Like:

Susan Rogers. That was, in a word, dope.

It Sounds Like:

Alright, Well, please be sure to join us next week when we are going to get to the

It Sounds Like:

bottom of the grand question, the question everybody wants to know.

It Sounds Like:

Why do humans even have music?

It Sounds Like:

So it's a fundamental human property.

It Sounds Like:

How and why it evolved is hard to discern from the fossil record, right?

It Sounds Like:

Because music doesn't leave fossils.

It Sounds Like:

Just the easiest question to ever try to answer.

It Sounds Like:

Should be pretty cut and dry. Probably going to be like, what?

It Sounds Like:

A two minute episode?

It Sounds Like:

So please make sure that you subscribe to

It Sounds Like:

the So Curious podcast everywhere you listen.

It Sounds Like:

And please leave us a five star review so we can get out to more people.

It Sounds Like:

And please make sure you don't miss it.

It Sounds Like:

Tuesdays be there.

It Sounds Like:

5AM sharp!

It Sounds Like:

This podcast is made in partnership with

It Sounds Like:

Radio Kismet, Philadelphia's premier podcast production studio.

It Sounds Like:

This podcast is produced by Amy Carson.

It Sounds Like:

The Franklin Institute's Director of Digital Editorial is Joy Montefusco.

It Sounds Like:

Dr.

It Sounds Like:

Jayatri Das is the Franklin Institute's Chief Bioscientist.

It Sounds Like:

And Erin Armstrong runs Marketing, Communications and Digital Media.

It Sounds Like:

Head of Operations is Christopher Plant.

It Sounds Like:

Our mixing engineer is Justin Berger and our audio editor is Lauren DeLuca.

It Sounds Like:

Our graphic designer is Emma Seeger.

It Sounds Like:

And I'm The Bul Bey!

It Sounds Like:

And I'm Kirsten Michelle Cills. Thanks!

It Sounds Like:

Thank you! See ya!

Chapters