Language is the basis for all human connection. It allows us to communicate our ideas, express our feelings, and have discussions with the people in our lives. The language skills we develop as children lay the foundation for how we communicate as adults. Typically developing children begin speaking between 12-18 months and start regularly incorporating verbs around the age of 2. Research suggests that the number of verbs a child knows at age 2 is an indicator of their grammar skills at age 3. But what happens when these language skills are delayed?
In this episode, Dr. Sabrina Horvath an assistant professor of rehabilitation sciences in the College of Health Professions at MUSC shares her research in childhood language development. Horvath is the Principal Investigator for the MACAW Lab, which stands for Methods of Acquiring Concepts and Words. Horvath leads research in child language development and disorder, late talkers, and vocabulary development.
Episode Links:
Have an idea for an episode of Science Never Sleeps? Send us an email at ScienceNeverSleeps@musc.edu.
1
::From the Medical University of South Carolina,
this is Science Never Sleeps, a show that explores
2
::the science, the people, and the stories behind
the scenes of biomedical research happening
3
::at MUSC. I'm Gwen Bouchie. Have you ever stopped
to think about the role language plays in our
4
::lives? From casual greetings to public speeches,
language shapes our interactions, perceptions,
5
::and identities. The language skills we use as
adults begin to form in our earliest years.
6
::and we carry these skills with us throughout
our lifetimes. But what happens when children
7
::have delays in language development during those
formative years? How can we course correct
8
::to make sure that children develop language
skills that will set them up for a future of
9
::success? In this episode of Science Never Sleeps,
we're joined by Dr. Sabrina Horvath, an assistant
10
::professor of rehabilitation sciences in the
College of Health Professions at MUSC. Dr.
11
::Horvath is also the principal investigator for
the MACAW Lab. standing for methods of acquiring
12
::concepts and words. The MACAW Lab explores
how children develop their vocabularies and
13
::broader language skills and the importance of
verbs in later language development. Stay with
14
::us.
15
::Dr. Horvath, welcome to Science Never Sleeps.
Thank you so much for having me. It's a real
16
::pleasure. So you are a speech pathologist. And
I want to really jump in first. One of the
17
::things we talk about on Science Never Sleeps
are researchers' journeys. How do they get
18
::where they are? And you have this really fascinating
journey of starting in your early career, your
19
::early college career. just in linguistics and
history, just sort of immersed in language,
20
::and then that brought you to where you are now.
So I would love to hear you talk a little bit
21
::about what that journey has been like for you
and how you got to where you are today. Absolutely.
22
::Well, as you mentioned, I started my undergraduate
career as actually a history major and a Spanish-German
23
::double minor. But more broadly, I've just always
loved and been fascinated by languages. I grew
24
::up just outside of Washington, D.C., which is
a beautifully diverse area to live, and had
25
::several friends growing up who spoke languages
other than English at home. And it's the sort
26
::of place where you go to the grocery store and
hear half a dozen different languages on your
27
::way to get eggs. So I've always been exposed
to this idea of the richness of language and
28
::the variability of language, and the idea that
language connects us to others, to our own
29
::culture, and... through language we can experience
other cultures and other ideas that we might
30
::not be familiar with from our own particular
experiences. So that was my motivation for
31
::studying two languages as minors, but I very
quickly found and fell in love with linguistics,
32
::which is the study of language in a scientific
way. How do we put words together and make
33
::sentences, and how might that look different
from one language to another? What are the
34
::rules that govern how we... can use and formulate
sentences socially, structurally in the language
35
::itself. And importantly to my future work, how
do children ever seem to figure this out? Because
36
::it's a very complicated system. And yet so many
children seem to do it effortlessly without
37
::any sort of direct instruction. So this was
kind of the beginning of my journey. And speech
38
::pathology, of course, is one very obvious pathway
from linguistics and one that I was drawn to
39
::because of those clinical implications of helping
children for whom this process is not unfolding
40
::as it should, for whom there are challenges.
So I got actually the history and linguistics
41
::double major. The history has not served me
particularly well moving forward, but I went
42
::straight into a master's program in speech language
pathology. did a little bit of research as
43
::part of that because I was interested in the
how and the why and worked clinically for a
44
::couple of years before deciding that research
was really where I wanted to be. So went back,
45
::got my PhD, did a postdoc and was very, very
lucky to end up here at MUSC for a faculty
46
::position. And here you are. And you're working
now with children, particularly toddlers. talking
47
::about your work today, I wanted to define a toddler.
So when we talk about toddlers and the kids
48
::that you're working with, what does that mean?
What is that age range? What are we looking
49
::at? Absolutely. Most of my research is from
the two to three age range. But I've run studies
50
::with children as young as about 18 months as
part of my work. and as old as about seven
51
::years of age, relevant to some of my, depending
on what the question is, of course. Right,
52
::right, depending on the research. Yes. Yeah.
With two to three year olds, what we're seeing
53
::a lot of is this rapid growth in vocabulary
development and the beginnings of putting words
54
::and short sentences together. So I'm interested
in how this process is happening and why some
55
::children might be struggling with this. And
the children who struggle with this, we typically
56
::refer to as late talkers. And so we do, so many
of us have either children that we know in
57
::our lives who have been late talkers or perhaps
ourselves we were late talkers. So this really
58
::is sort of a common thing across lots of different
children, I would imagine. Absolutely. Late
59
::talking impacts about 12 to 13 percent of kids.
And late talking itself is not a diagnosis.
60
::However, children who are late talkers are at
increased risk for ultimately having a diagnosis
61
::of something called developmental language disorder.
Developmental language disorder is a disorder
62
::in which an individual has difficulty understanding
and using language. And that challenge is not
63
::attributable to something like autism spectrum
disorder. or cerebral palsy, or an intellectual
64
::disability. These challenges can manifest in
many ways, such as difficulty finding the words
65
::that they want to say, being unspecific in their
word choices, or perhaps having difficulty
66
::following complex directions or understanding
figurative language. And as I said, it's the
67
::most common disorder you've probably never heard
of. It impacts about 7% to 8% of the population.
68
::So this is. about three times more common than
autism spectrum disorder and about as common
69
::as ADHD. Wow, yeah. So our goal in the McCall
lab is to first, help all late talkers who
70
::could all benefit from therapy services, but
second, to try to identify which of our late
71
::talkers are at greatest risk for ultimately
having developmental language disorder, so
72
::that we know who needs the greatest support
in those preschool years. because early intervention
73
::is critical in improving long-term outcomes.
And as part of your work, you really have this
74
::focus on verbs. And so what is it about the
verb in language that makes it such a critical
75
::piece of assessing language skill? That is such
a good question and one I love talking about.
76
::Verbs are unique because they bridge the gap
between the vocabulary, words and their meanings,
77
::and the structure of language itself. So it's
the verb that actually determines what a sentence
78
::is going to look like. For example, the verb
sleep can only take a subject. I sleep, you
79
::sleep, the dog sleeps. Comparatively, the verb
give. must take a subject and an object and
80
::an indirect object. I give you the book, for
example. So the verb is really what dictates
81
::what can and needs to appear in that larger
sentence structure. The reason I focus on verbs
82
::is because this is potentially a very early
way of getting a sense of children's later
83
::grammatical skills and development. And that
grammar piece is what children who have developmental
84
::language disorder struggle most with. Unfortunately,
we can't back up our current age of diagnosis
85
::for developmental language disorder, which is
about four to five, because we've traditionally
86
::been waiting for these milestones that typically
developing children have to hit, where we can
87
::then compare and say the child isn't doing X,
Y, and Z. Therefore, they have developmental
88
::language disorder. I think if we focus on verbs,
we can predict which children are struggling,
89
::even in the earliest stages, with what will
become their grammatical system. Verbs are
90
::also interesting because learning verbs themselves
requires some knowledge of grammar. So we're
91
::getting insight into their learning process,
their ability to use grammar at an early age,
92
::and then when they learn verbs, they're reinforcing
what they know of grammar and expanding their
93
::ability to use structure and language. So the
idea is then that if we provide intervention
94
::focusing on verbs, we're supporting not only
vocabulary development. but some of those grammatical
95
::skills as well. And it reminds me a little of,
if you think about the early primers, see,
96
::spot, run, those type of things are very basic
sentence structure as children are learning.
97
::So it really is, they're learning from that
very young age, the sentence structure of how
98
::to tie the action of something or someone together
through language. And your hope is to try to
99
::assess their skill or ability to be able to
do that or not do that much earlier so that
100
::they can get intervention sooner. One of the
things we do know about verb learning is that
101
::at very, very young ages, children are sensitive
to these patterns in language and use those
102
::patterns to make predictions about what new
words will mean. So this is a process for verb
103
::learning known as syntactic bootstrapping. Children
will assume if we teach them a made up verb
104
::that the duck is gorping the bunny means something
very different from the duck and the bunny
105
::are gorping. And so this attention to early
structure is really important in them being
106
::able to build their verb vocabularies. or I
should say, typically developing children,
107
::being able to build their verb vocabularies,
because we don't know if late talkers can use
108
::this skill or not. So talk a little bit about
your work in terms of the specificity around
109
::typical development versus some development
that falls outside of that typical range. So
110
::we would be thinking about children with autism,
perhaps other disorders. Can you talk a little
111
::bit about what you're exploring in that space?
Absolutely. Most of my work is comparing typically
112
::developing children to late talkers, but I've
also done some work with children on the autism
113
::spectrum as well. Autism is a unique disorder
to look at when we think of language because
114
::language challenges are no longer part of the
diagnosis itself. However, the majority of
115
::children on the autism spectrum do have some
challenge with language. We are interested
116
::in the relationship between those characteristic
features of autistic children, like the challenges
117
::they have with joint attention and social engagement,
and what those implications might be for the
118
::language learning process. I'm sure a day in
your lab or in your study environment is wildly
119
::fun because I can't imagine, you know, having
these toddlers coming in and their families.
120
::So let's talk a little bit about what is a typical
study visit look like for the kids who are
121
::coming in to work with you? Well, as you said,
we try to make it as fun as possible for the
122
::families and their children. We know it can
be a challenge to drive into downtown Charleston,
123
::but we want to make sure that the visit is enjoyable
and a positive experience for everyone. We
124
::always start with a little play to make sure
that the child feels comfortable in the space
125
::and with the research team. And we also take
that time to sign consent forms, which are
126
::always important, and gather any additional
information that we might need from the family,
127
::such as completing a vocabulary checklist of
the words that they know, or maybe providing
128
::some demographic information for us. The actual
study will vary based on our particular research
129
::questions and goals, but a lot of our studies
use eye tracking methodology, which is a lot
130
::of fun for children and their families. Our
children sit in our special race car chair
131
::in front of an eye tracking machine where we
display videos and it captures in real time
132
::the exact coordinates on the screen where the
child is looking. And we can analyze this data.
133
::to get a lot of information about language skills,
language processing, language knowledge, and
134
::really a far more robust data set than we would
get from just pointing, for example. Yeah,
135
::the eye tracking is really interesting because
it does give you that extra set of data. And
136
::what are we really looking for when we're looking
at eye tracking? That's a great question. One
137
::thing we can look at is accuracy. So when we
run studies that have made up verbs, we can
138
::look at whether the child can, for example,
find groping when we ask them to do so. Another
139
::thing we can look at is something called language
processing speed. And for these, we're using
140
::familiar words. We might display a picture of
a ball and a shoe and say, find the ball. And
141
::what's very interesting is the speed at which
a child will look to the ball is predictive
142
::not only of their concurrent vocabulary size,
but also their language growth and later language
143
::outcomes. At least if you're looking at nouns,
we've been exploring this in verb vocabulary
144
::as well. So how is it that you can tie those
things together in terms of it being predictive
145
::of a future vocabulary? Well, these have been
longitudinal studies traditionally, where the
146
::children who are faster to find the item have
better language scores on standardized tests
147
::to age eight. We have worked on how to quantify
processing speed when children are watching
148
::videos of verb actions, as opposed to looking
at static images, because eye behaviors will
149
::change if you're looking at a video versus an
image. Right now we've only done concurrent
150
::studies. We've done one with late talkers and
one with autistic children. But our hope in
151
::the future is to do some longitudinal work to
see if this processing of verb vocabulary also
152
::predicts later language outcomes. One of the
things that we found is that late talkers and
153
::typically developing children have different
types of verbs in their early verb vocabularies.
154
::So I'll explain what that means. We've looked
at several different aspects of how verbs encode
155
::meaning. And one that is important across the
world's languages is manner versus result verbs.
156
::Manner verbs are verbs that encode how something
happens, like walk, run, jump, hop, skip. These
157
::are all manners of motion. Result verbs, by
contrast, encode the end state of the event,
158
::like open or close or finish. What we've found,
very interestingly, is that late talkers have
159
::a preference for result over manner meanings
even from the earliest stages of development,
160
::whereas typically developing children prefer
manner over result verbs. So this is a notable
161
::difference. We've also found that irrespective
of whether a child is or is not a late talker,
162
::Children who have proportionally more manner
than result verbs have larger vocabularies
163
::overall. So this has opened up several interesting
lines of research for us in thinking about
164
::why each group might prefer one verb type over
another and what it might mean for intervention
165
::in terms of picking therapy targets. Another
notable finding that we have out of our lab
166
::is actually publishing the first ever study
to look at verb learning mechanisms in late
167
::talkers. In this particular study, we were considering
whether it was better for late talkers and
168
::typically developing children when learning
a new verb to hear only content nouns, the
169
::boy is daxing the balloon, the boy is daxing
the balloon, or to hear a mix of content nouns
170
::and pronouns, the boy is daxing the balloon,
he is daxing it. We found, somewhat to our
171
::surprise, that both typically developing children
and late talkers performed better given only
172
::content nouns. But late talkers in particular
performed very poorly when they were given
173
::the suboptimal content noun pronoun mix. So
this is evidence for the idea that when a verb
174
::learning situation is suboptimal, late talkers
are particularly negatively impacted. And further
175
::support. for the idea that we need to be conscientious
about how we're teaching new vocabulary items
176
::to children who are already struggling with
vocabulary. So let's talk for a second about
177
::these made up words. You have mentioned a few,
and so I just, it's so fascinating that there's
178
::these, you know, just made up words that you're
working with in your lab. So what is the significance
179
::of these words and working with the kids with
these particular words? Well, our purpose in
180
::using made-up words is to make sure that the
child doesn't already know what that word is.
181
::That way we can say confidently that when they
demonstrate knowledge of this made-up word,
182
::it's not because they came in already knowing
what daxing or gorping or ziffing is. And in
183
::the actual experiments, we're able to make subtle
changes to learning environments. For example.
184
::comparing only content nouns versus content
nouns and pronouns, and make conclusions from
185
::our experiment about which learning situations
are optimal or suboptimal. This is one of the
186
::major goals of our research because identifying
what is optimal is necessary when we think
187
::about designing efficacious interventions. Do
you think there are implications for this research
188
::to... for... is generally the way we teach language
to children? Absolutely. And I think you're
189
::tapping into a really interesting question about
our perceptions of how much teaching needs
190
::to happen in the process of language development.
Interestingly, there are cultures across the
191
::globe where it's inappropriate socially to talk
to a child before the child is at a stage where
192
::they can be conversational in return. children
learn language beautifully in these cultures
193
::as well. What we see is that the child mind,
whether it's pre-programmed for language or
194
::not, a theoretical debate for another time,
is ready to embrace the rich complex system
195
::that is language to work through it, make sense
of it, and then employ it themselves. in a
196
::variety of circumstances and with a variety
of experiences. Our challenge, particularly,
197
::is for those children who struggle with this
process and making sure that we can support
198
::them as best we are able to improve their long-term
outcomes. With your study, you're looking at
199
::things like verbal cues, various different methods
of doing this assessment. Can you tell us a
200
::little bit about what that looks like and maybe
where that comes from? Figuring out what an
201
::unfamiliar word means is actually an incredibly
difficult task. And sometimes we don't think
202
::about how hard it is for children to make sense
of a word that they have never heard before.
203
::There's been wonderful research over the past
60 to 70 years to identify some of the cues
204
::that children use when making sense of an unfamiliar
word. One of the cues is visual information.
205
::It's a lot easier to figure out what a word
means if the referent of that word is visually
206
::available to the child in real time. Children
also use social cues to figure out what words
207
::mean. For example, a parent might point to an
item or direct the child's attention in some
208
::other way to bring attention to the word that
they are teaching. One cue that's incredibly
209
::important, but perhaps not so immediately obvious,
is using linguistic information to figure out
210
::what that word means. And there are all sorts
of linguistic cues that children use. For example,
211
::by about 18 months of age, children can reliably
class words into nouns or verbs based on how
212
::they're used in a sentence. If they hear, a ziff, a mody, they know that that's probably
213
::a noun. Whereas if they hear gorp-ing or zeb-ing,
they know that that's probably a verb. They're
214
::also aware by this age that nouns denote objects
and verbs denote actions. So this is one way
215
::they may narrow down their hypotheses of what
an unfamiliar word might mean. When we consider
216
::verb learning specifically, Typically developing
children use a process known as syntactic bootstrapping.
217
::This means they use the structure of the sentence
and the relationship of the nouns to the verb
218
::to make inferences about the type of meaning
encoded by that verb. I'll give you an example.
219
::Typically developing children by age two know
that the duck is groping the bunny means that
220
::the duck is doing something to. a bunny. Conversely,
the duck and the bunny are groping means that
221
::the duck is doing something and the bunny is
doing something separately from that duck.
222
::We actually don't know if late talkers use syntactic
bootstrapping at all in their process of verb
223
::learning. This is one of the studies we're running
right now in the lab, and I'm very excited
224
::to see what the results are. There are different
implications about the verb learning process
225
::and intervention techniques based on whether
this cue is available to late talkers in learning
226
::new verbs. Do you have any tips for parents
of toddlers or even of really honestly parents
227
::at any age probably, you know, kids sub five,
I guess, based on your work and what you know
228
::particularly as a researcher and also as a speech
language pathologist? This is a question I
229
::get asked a lot. The first recommendation I
make to any parent is to read to your child.
230
::Reading is a wonderful way to expand a child's
vocabulary and their grammar skills, because
231
::we use grammatical structures and vocabulary
items in literature, even children's literature,
232
::that we don't use in our day-to-day conversation.
For example, ball means one thing when we're
233
::playing outside and another thing altogether
when reading Cinderella. Another thing that
234
::I encourage parents to do is to talk to and
with and around their children. You are enriching
235
::their language environment by giving them exposure
to language even if you're not talking directly
236
::to them. Narrating what you're doing or narrating
what you're seeing is a wonderful way to spark
237
::their language growth. I also get a lot of questions
about language development from parents who
238
::are concerned that their child might be a late
talker. It's hard to know what the milestones
239
::are. To any parent who asks me, I always recommend
that they talk with their pediatrician. If
240
::you have serious concerns, it is worth it to
follow up and potentially pursue an evaluation.
241
::Why are things like the number of verbs that
a child knows and sentence structure important?
242
::These are parts of language known as morphology
and syntax. And together, morphology and syntax
243
::make grammar. They're so important because children
who have developmental language disorder struggle
244
::with grammar particularly. We're interested
in verbs as an early predictor of where they'll
245
::end up with their grammar skills and who might
struggle the most. But I've used these very
246
::technical terms, morphology and syntax, and
I feel like I should define them a little bit
247
::more. Syntax is essentially the word order of
a language. For example, in English, we have
248
::a subject verb object word order. In other languages
like Hungarian, the verb goes at the end of
249
::the sentence. Morphology are these little pieces
of language that let us know who did what to
250
::whom and when it happened. They don't have any
meaning in and of themselves other than conveying
251
::this information. Examples in English include
the progressive -ing, walking, eating, the past
252
::tense -ed, walked, talked, or the plural s, shoes,
socks. What we've found is that the number
253
::of verbs a child has at age two predicts how
many morphemes they know at age three and how
254
::complex their sentences are. We find this very
suggestive of the role that verbs play in developing
255
::grammar. So understanding how children learn
verbs and how to possibly improve that learning.
256
::is key to their language success in the future.
Absolutely. We've been talking to Dr. Sabrina
257
::Horvath about childhood language development.
Have an idea for a future episode of Science
258
::Never Sleeps? Click on the link in the show
notes to share with us. Science Never Sleeps
259
::is produced by the Office of the Vice President
for Research at the Medical University of South
260
::Carolina. Special thanks to the Office of Instructional
Technology for support on this episode.