Proudly sponsored by PyMC Labs, the Bayesian Consultancy. Book a call, or get in touch!
I love Bayesian modeling. Not only because it allows me to model interesting phenomena and learn about the world I live in. But because it’s part of a broader epistemological framework that confronts me with deep questions — how do you make decisions under uncertainty? How do you communicate risk and uncertainty? What does being rational even mean?
Thankfully, Gerd Gigerenzer is there to help us navigate these fascinating topics. Gerd is the Director of the Harding Center for Risk Literacy of the University of Potsdam, Germany.
Also Director emeritus at the Max Planck Institute for Human Development, he is a former Professor of Psychology at the University of Chicago and Distinguished Visiting Professor at the School of Law of the University of Virginia.
Gerd has written numerous awarded articles and books, including Risk Savvy, Simple Heuristics That Make Us Smart, Rationality for Mortals, and How to Stay Smart in a Smart World.
As you’ll hear, Gerd has trained U.S. federal judges, German physicians, and top managers to make better decisions under uncertainty.
But Gerd is also a banjo player, has won a medal in Judo, and loves scuba diving, skiing, and, above all, reading.
Our theme music is « Good Bayesian », by Baba Brinkman (feat MC Lars and Mega Ran). Check out his awesome work at https://bababrinkman.com/ !
Thank you to my Patrons for making this episode possible!
Yusuke Saito, Avi Bryant, Ero Carrera, Giuliano Cruz, Tim Gasser, James Wade, Tradd Salvo, William Benton, James Ahloy, Robin Taylor,, Chad Scherrer, Zwelithini Tunyiswa, Bertrand Wilden, James Thompson, Stephen Oates, Gian Luca Di Tanna, Jack Wells, Matthew Maldonado, Ian Costley, Ally Salim, Larry Gill, Ian Moran, Paul Oreto, Colin Caprani, Colin Carroll, Nathaniel Burbank, Michael Osthege, Rémi Louf, Clive Edelsten, Henri Wallen, Hugo Botha, Vinh Nguyen, Marcin Elantkowski, Adam C. Smith, Will Kurt, Andrew Moskowitz, Hector Munoz, Marco Gorelli, Simon Kessell, Bradley Rode, Patrick Kelley, Rick Anderson, Casper de Bruin, Philippe Labonde, Michael Hankin, Cameron Smith, Tomáš Frýda, Ryan Wesslen, Andreas Netti, Riley King, Yoshiyuki Hamajima, Sven De Maeyer, Michael DeCrescenzo, Fergal M, Mason Yahr, Naoya Kanai, Steven Rowland, Aubrey Clayton, Jeannine Sue, Omri Har Shemesh, Scott Anthony Robson, Robert Yolken, Or Duek, Pavel Dusek, Paul Cox, Andreas Kröpelin, Raphaël R, Nicolas Rode, Gabriel Stechschulte, Arkady, Kurt TeKolste, Gergely Juhasz, Marcus Nölke, Maggi Mackintosh, Grant Pezzolesi, Avram Aelony, Joshua Meehl, Javier Sabio, Kristian Higgins, Alex Jones, Gregorio Aguilar, Matt Rosinski, Bart Trudeau and Luis Fonseca.
Visit https://www.patreon.com/learnbayesstats to unlock exclusive Bayesian swag ;)
Links from the show:
Abstract
In this episode, we have no other than Gerd Gigerenzer on the show, an expert in decision making, rationality and communicating risk and probabilities.
Gerd is a trained psychologist and worked at a number of distinguished institutes like the Max Planck Institute for Human Development in Berlin or the University of Chicago. He is director of the Harding Center for Risk Literacy in Potsdam.
One of his many topics of study are heuristics, a term often misunderstood, as he explains. We talk about the role of heuristics in a world of uncertainty, how it interacts with analysis and how it relates to intuition.
Another major topic of his work and this episode are natural frequencies and how they are a more natural way than conditional probabilities to express information such as the probability of having cancer after a positive screening.
Gerd studied the usefulness of natural frequencies in practice and contributed to them being taught in high school in Bavaria, Germany, as an important tool to navigate the real world.
In general, Gerd is passionate about not only researching these topics but also seeing them applied outside of academia. He taught thousands of medical doctors how to understand and communicate statistics and also worked on a number of economical decision making scenarios.
In the end we discuss the benefits of simpler models for complex, uncertain situations, as for example in the case of predicting flu seasons.
Transcript
This is an automatic transcript and may therefore contain errors. Please get in touch if you're willing to correct them.
Gert Gigerentzer, welcome to Learning
Vision Statistics.
2
:I'm glad to be here.
3
:Yeah, thanks a lot for taking the time.
4
:I am very happy to have you on the show.
5
:A few patrons have asked for your episode,
so I'm glad to have you here today.
6
:And thank you very much to all of you in
the Slack, in the LBS Slack who
7
:recommended Gert for an episode on the
show.
8
:And yeah, I have a lot of questions for
you because you've done a lot of things.
9
:You have a lot of, there is a lot of
questions I want to ask you on a lot of
10
:different topics, but first, as usual,
let's start with your origin story.
11
:Geert, and basically, how did you come to
the world of study of rationality and
12
:decision-making under uncertainty?
13
:Now, I have been observing myself, how I
make decisions.
14
:For instance, in an earlier career, I was
a musician playing dixieland, jazz, and
15
:other things.
16
:And when I did my PhD work, I had to make
a decision.
17
:Was I want to continue a career on the
stage as a musician or to try an academic
18
:career?
19
:Mm-hmm.
20
:And for me, music was the safe option,
because I knew, and also I earned much
21
:more money than an assistant professor.
22
:And an academic career, I couldn't know
whether I could make it, whether I would
23
:ever become a professor, but it was the
risky option.
24
:So this is, if you want an initial story,
I decided then to take the uncertainty at
25
:risk.
26
:That makes sense.
27
:And so that was like pretty early in your
career, or is that something that came
28
:later on when you already had started
studying other things, or you started
29
:doing that as soon as you started your
undergrad studies?
30
:What came later was that I learned about
theories about decision making, and some
31
:of them I found very unrealistic and
strange, and about topics that were not
32
:really the topics where I thought are
important, like which job do you take,
33
:what do you do with the rest of your life,
but were of monetary gambles, was it you
34
:want a hundred dollars for sure, or two
hundred with a probability of 0.4?
35
:or six.
36
:And I also spent an important year of my
life at the Center for Interdisciplinary
37
:Research in Bielefeld on a group called
the Probabilistic Revolution.
38
:That's an international and
interdisciplinary group that investigated
39
:how science changed from a deterministic
worldview to a probabilistic one.
40
:And I learned so much.
41
:I was one of the young guys in this group.
42
:There were people like Thomas Kuhn, Ian
Hacking, Nancy Cartwright.
43
:And that also taught me something.
44
:It's important not to read in your own
discipline and do what the others do.
45
:But to fall in love is a topic like
decision making and uncertainty in the
46
:real world.
47
:And then read everything.
48
:that people have written about that.
49
:And that means from areas like biology,
animal behavior, to economics, to
50
:sociology, to the history of science.
51
:Yeah, that was something really
interesting when preparing the episode
52
:with you to see the whole arc of your
career being basically around these topics
53
:that you've studied really a lot and
in-depth.
54
:So that was really super interesting to
notice.
55
:And so something I'm wondering is, if you
remember...
56
:how you first got introduced to Bayesian
methods.
57
:Now, for instance, I read Fisher's book,
Statistic Methods and
58
:Mm-hmm.
59
:Thomas Bayes for having the insight not to
publishing his paper.
60
:Because, according to Fisher, that's not
what you need in science.
61
:And I got very much interested in the
fights between statisticians, in something
62
:that could be called insult and injury.
63
:And Fisher, for instance, in the same
book, he destroys Carl Pearson, his
64
:successor, saying
65
:the terrible weakness of his mathematical
and scientific work flowed from his
66
:incapacity of self-criticism.
67
:So if you want to get anyone interested in
statistics, then start with the
68
:controversies.
69
:That's my advice.
70
:And the pity is that in the textbooks, in
psychology certainly,
71
:All the controversies have been
eliminated, one doesn't mention them, and
72
:talks as if there would be only one kind
of statistics.
73
:So that could be Fisher's null hypothesis
testing, which has been turned in a very
74
:strange ritual, Fisher never would accept,
or on the other side there are also
75
:Bayesians who think it's the only tool in
the toolbox.
76
:And the knees of that attitude is
realistic, it's more religious.
77
:There is a statistical toolbox.
78
:And there are different instruments and
you need to look at the problem to choose
79
:the right one.
80
:And also within bass, there are so many
different kinds of bassianism.
81
:There's not one.
82
:64,000.
83
:It's a lot.
84
:Yeah, so, okay, that makes it clear.
85
:And that helps me also understand your
work because, yeah, something I saw is in
86
:your work, you often emphasize the role of
heuristics in decision-making.
87
:So I'm curious if you could explain how
Bayesian thinking and heuristics intersect
88
:and...
89
:how do these approaches complement each
other in navigating uncertainty?
90
:First, the term heuristic is often
misunderstood.
91
:I mean the term in the sense that Herbert
Simon used it to make a computer program
92
:smart, or the Gestalt psychologist used
it, or Einstein used it in the title of
93
:his Nobel Prize winning paper of 1905.
94
:I don't use it in the sense that it has
been very popular in psychology and other
95
:fields.
96
:as heuristics and biases.
97
:That's a clear misunderstanding.
98
:So to make it very short, in a world that
Jimmy Savage, who is often called the
99
:father of Bayesian statistics, called a
small world where the entire state space
100
:is known and nothing else can happen.
101
:In that world,
102
:This is the ideal world for Bayesianism
and also for most of statistics.
103
:In a world where you do not know the state
space that the economist Frank Knight
104
:called uncertainty, or as I have called
true uncertainty or radical uncertainty,
105
:you can't optimize by definition.
106
:You cannot find the best solution.
107
:And here...
108
:People and other animals, just like
managers and scientists, use heuristics.
109
:So a heuristic is a rule that helps you,
under uncertainty, to find a good
110
:solution.
111
:For instance, Polia, the mathematician
distinguished between analysis and
112
:heuristics.
113
:You need heuristics to find a proof and
you need analysis to check.
114
:whether it was right.
115
:Most important, heuristics and analysis
are not opposites, as it's now become very
116
:popular in system one and system two
theories.
117
:They're not opposites.
118
:They go together.
119
:And for instance, a study of 17 noble
laureates reported that almost all of them
120
:attributed there.
121
:success from going back and forth between
heuristics slash intuition or analysis.
122
:So that's an important thing.
123
:It's not binary opposites.
124
:So your question, where does Bayes meet
heuristics?
125
:Now, of course, for instance, in the
determination of the prior probability
126
:distribution, uniform
127
:That's also known as one over N.
128
:So you divide, for instance, your assets
equally over the funds or the stocks that
129
:you have.
130
:It's a reasonable assumption when you know
little.
131
:And just as one over n is reasonable, in
some situations it's not always.
132
:And the real challenge is to find out in
what situation does a certain heuristic or
133
:does space work, and where does it not
work.
134
:That's what I call the study of ecological
rationality.
135
:So in short, there's no single tool that's
always the best.
136
:We need to face...
137
:The difficult question, can we identify
the structure of environments where a
138
:simple heuristic like equal distribution
or imitate others works and where does it
139
:mislead?
140
:Hehehe
141
:Yeah, yeah, this is really interesting
because something also I'm always like, I
142
:always try to reconcile and actually you
talk about it in your book, Gut Feelings,
143
:The Intelligence of the Unconscious.
144
:And you talk also about intuitions and how
they can sometimes outperform more complex
145
:analytical processes.
146
:And this is a claim that you can see in a
lot of fields, right?
147
:From, I don't know, politics to medicine
to sports, when basically people don't
148
:really want the analytical process to be
taken too seriously because maybe it
149
:doesn't go, it doesn't confirm their...
150
:Yeah.
151
:their previous analysis or their own bias.
152
:So what I'm wondering is how do Bayesian
methods in your research, how do Bayesian
153
:methods accommodate the role of intuitive
judgment and how can individuals strike a
154
:balance between intuitive thinking and the
systematic updating of beliefs that we use
155
:under Bayesian reasoning?
156
:So let me first define what I mean by
intuition.
157
:So intuition is a kind of unconscious
intelligence that is based on years of
158
:experience with a topic where one feels
quickly what one should do, what one
159
:should not do, but one cannot explain it.
160
:So when a doctor sees a patient and the
doctor may feel something is wrong with
161
:that patient but cannot explain it, that's
an intuition based on years of experience.
162
:And then the doctor will go on and do
tests and analysis in order to find out
163
:what's wrong if there's something.
164
:So remember, intuition and analysis are
the same.
165
:always go together.
166
:It's a big error what we have today in
so-called dual processing theories, where
167
:they're presented as opposites.
168
:And then usually one side is always right,
like analysis and intuition is blamed, and
169
:heuristics are blamed if things go wrong.
170
:I see.
171
:Yeah.
172
:And so how does that then integrate into
the Bayesian framework according to you?
173
:Like in the systematic analysis of beliefs
that we have in the Bayesian framework.
174
:So applications of Bayes use heuristics
such as 1 over n, so equal distribution,
175
:equal priors.
176
:And they also use a more silent
independence assumption and such things.
177
:But I would not phrase the problem as how
to integrate heuristics in the Bayesian
178
:framework.
179
:I would also not say...
180
:how to integrate Bayes in the heuristics
framework.
181
:I think of both, so there are many
Bayesian methods and also other
182
:statistical methods, the old optimizing
methods, and there are heuristic methods
183
:which are non-optimizing methods.
184
:I think of them as part of an adaptive
toolbox that humans have, that they can
185
:use, and the real art is the choice of the
right.
186
:tool.
187
:So when I should use base and what kind of
base or when should I use a heuristic, a
188
:social heuristic, for instance do what
Alex tells me to do or for instance simple
189
:heuristics like take the best which just
go lexicographically through reasons and
190
:stop with the first one that allows to
make a decision.
191
:And that's the question of ecological
rationality.
192
:I see.
193
:And do you have, yeah, do you have
examples?
194
:Bayes' rule is a rule that is reasonable
to apply in situations where the world is
195
:stable, where no unexpected things happen,
where you have good estimates for the
196
:priors and also good estimates for the
likelihoods.
197
:For instance, mammography screening is a
case.
198
:So...
199
:We know that the, or we can expect that
the results of mammography screening won't
200
:change very much.
201
:We have to take in account that the base
rates differ from country to country or
202
:from group to group.
203
:But besides that, it is a good framework
to understand what is the probability that
204
:a person has breast cancer.
205
:if she tests positive.
206
:Mm-hmm.
207
:But that's a good situation.
208
:But if you have something which is highly
volatile, like, okay, I worked with the
209
:Bank of England on a method for
regulation, for banking regulation, and
210
:that role is highly volatile, and you're
not getting very far with standard
211
:statistical methods.
212
:But you may evaluate whether a bank is in
troubles.
213
:by something that we call a fast and
frugal tree that only looks at maybe three
214
:or four important variables and doesn't
combine them in a way as base or as linear
215
:models do, but lexicographic.
216
:Why?
217
:Because, so if you first look, for
instance, think about medical diagnosis.
218
:If your heart fails, a good kidney cannot
compensate that.
219
:And this is the idea of lexicographic
models.
220
:And a number of heuristics are
lexicographic, as opposed to compensatory
221
:models like Bayes or linear regressions.
222
:Oh, I see, okay.
223
:Yeah, continue.
224
:Yeah, I have myself trained about a
thousand doctors in understanding and
225
:doing Bayesian diagnosis and Bayesian
thinking.
226
:And you should realize that most doctors
and also most gynecologists would not be
227
:able to answer the question I posed
before.
228
:What is the...
229
:probability that a woman has breast cancer
in screening when the mammogram is
230
:positive.
231
:And if I give them the numbers in
conditional probabilities, they're equally
232
:lost.
233
:Alex, I do a test with you.
234
:Are you ready?
235
:So the point will be, I give you the
information in, as usual, in conditional
236
:probabilities.
237
:And I hope you will be confused.
238
:And also, to readers, the listeners.
239
:And then I give you the same.
240
:information in what we call natural
frequencies.
241
:And then insight will come.
242
:Ready?
243
:Okay.
244
:So assume you conduct a mammography
screening.
245
:What you know is that among the group of
women who participates, there is a one
246
:percent chance that a woman has breast
cancer undetected.
247
:You also know that the probability that a
woman has positive if she
248
:as breast cancer is 90%.
249
:And you know that the probability that
women should test positive if she does not
250
:have breast cancer is 9%.
251
:Okay?
252
:You have a base rate of 1%, a sensitivity
or hit rate of 90%, and a falls alarm rate
253
:of 9%.
254
:Now a woman in that group just tested
positive and you know nothing.
255
:about her because it's creamy, ask you,
doctor, tell me, do I now have breast
256
:cancer?
257
:Or how certain is it?
258
:99%, 90, 50, please tell me.
259
:What do you say?
260
:If there is now fog in your mind, that's
the typical situation of most doctors.
261
:Mm-hmm.
262
:And there have been conclusions made in
psychological research that the human mind
263
:has not evolved to think statistically, or
here, the Bayesian way.
264
:Now the problem is not in the mind, the
problem is in the representation of the
265
:information.
266
:Conditional probabilities are something
quite new.
267
:And few of us have been trained in it.
268
:Now how did humans...
269
:before Thomas Bass.
270
:Mm-hmm.
271
:or animals do based on reasoning, not
conditional probabilities, but what we
272
:call natural frequencies.
273
:That is, I give you first a demonstration,
then explain what it is.
274
:Okay, we use the same situation.
275
:You do the mammography screening and
translate the probabilities into concrete
276
:frequencies.
277
:Okay?
278
:Think about a hundred women.
279
:We expected one of them has breast cancer
and she likely tests positive.
280
:That's the 90%.
281
:Among the 99 who do not have breast
cancer, we expected another 9 will
282
:nevertheless test positive.
283
:So we have a total of 10 who test
positive.
284
:Question, how many of them do actually
have cancer?
285
:It's one out of 10.
286
:So a woman who tests positive in screening
has most likely not cancer.
287
:That's good news.
288
:So that's natural frequencies and you
basically see through.
289
:And natural frequencies, we call them
because they're not relative frequencies.
290
:They're not normalized.
291
:You start with a group like 100 and you
just break it down.
292
:And then the computation becomes very
simple.
293
:just imagine Bayes rule for this problem.
294
:And then natural frequencies does the
computation, the representation.
295
:It's just one out of the total number of
positives, 10.
296
:That's all.
297
:And once doctors have learned that and
tried with a few problems, they can
298
:generalize it and use the method for other
problems.
299
:And then we can avoid.
300
:the errors that are currently still in
place and also doctors can better
301
:understand what tests like HIV tests or
pregnancy tests actually mean.
302
:And the interesting theoretical point is,
as Herbert Simon said, the solution to the
303
:problem is in its representation.
304
:And he asked it from the Gestalt
Psychologist.
305
:Yeah, this is really interesting.
306
:I really love the...
307
:And in a way that's quite simple, right,
to just turn to natural frequencies.
308
:So I really love that because it gives a
simple solution to a problem that is
309
:indeed quite pronounced, right?
310
:Where it's just like when you're...
311
:Even if you're trained in statistics, you
have to make the conscious effort of not
312
:falling into the fallacy of...
313
:thinking, well, if the woman has a
positive test and the test has a 99% hit
314
:rate, she's got a 99% probability of
having breast cancer.
315
:I have one part of my brain which knows
that completely because I deal with
316
:statistics all the time, but there is
still the intuitive part of my brain,
317
:which is like, wait, why should I even
wonder if that's the true answer?
318
:So I like the fact that natural
frequencies
319
:kind of an elegant and simple solution to
that issue.
320
:And so I will put in the show notes your
paper about natural frequencies and also
321
:the one you've written about HIV screening
and how that relates to natural
322
:frequencies.
323
:So that's in the show notes for listeners.
324
:And I'm also curious, basically
concretely,
325
:how you did that with the professionals
you've collaborated with.
326
:Because your work has involved
collaborating with professionals from
327
:various domains.
328
:That means physicians, that means judges.
329
:I'm curious how you have applied these
principles of risk communication in
330
:practice with these professionals and what
challenges.
331
:and what successes have emerged from these
applications.
332
:Yeah, so I have always tried to connect my
theoretical work with practical work.
333
:So in that case of the doctors, I have
been teaching continuing medical education
334
:for doctors.
335
:So the courses that I give, they are
certified and the doctors gets points for
336
:that.
337
:and it may be a group of 150 or so doctors
who are assembled to a day or two days of
338
:continuing medical education, and I may do
two hours with them.
339
:And that has been for me a quite
satisfying experience because the doctors
340
:are grateful because they have muddled
through these things for their lives.
341
:And now they realize there's a simple
solution.
342
:They can learn within a half an hour or
so.
343
:And then it sticks for the rest of their
lives.
344
:I've also trained in the US, so I have
lived many years in the US and taught as a
345
:professor at the University of Chicago.
346
:And I have trained together with a program
from George Mason University, US Federal
347
:Churches.
348
:These are very smart people and I enjoyed
that.
349
:So these trainings were...
350
:and in illustrious places like Santa Fe.
351
:And the churches were included and their
partners also included.
352
:And there was also a series of things like
about how to understand fibers.
353
:And I was teaching them how to understand
risks and decision making and heuristics.
354
:And...
355
:If you think that federal churches who are
among the best ones in the US would
356
:understand Bayes' rule, good luck.
357
:No, there may be a few, most not.
358
:And actually, by the way, Bayes' rule is
forbidden in UK law.
359
:interesting.
360
:And so, but going back, these are examples
of training that every psychologist could
361
:do.
362
:But you have to leave your lab and go
outside and talk to doctors and have
363
:something to offer them for teaching.
364
:By now, the term natural frequencies is a
standard term in evidence-based medicine.
365
:And I'm very...
366
:proud about that.
367
:And many, there's also a review, a
Cochrane's review has looked at various
368
:representations and found that natural
frequencies are among the most powerful
369
:ones.
370
:And we have with some of our own students
who were more interested in children than
371
:in doctors, we have posed us the question,
can we teach children?
372
:and how early.
373
:And one of the papers I sent you, it's a
paper in the Journal of Experimental
374
:Psychology General, I think two years ago,
has for the first time tested fourth
375
:graders, fifth graders, sixth graders, and
second graders.
376
:So when we did this with the teachers,
they were saying, and they were looking at
377
:the problems,
378
:They were saying, no, that's much too
difficult.
379
:The children will not be able to do that.
380
:They haven't even had fractions.
381
:But you don't need fractions.
382
:And for instance, when we use problems,
they are more childlike.
383
:So here we put that type of problems.
384
:And when they are in natural frequencies,
385
:And the numbers are two-digit numbers.
386
:You can't do larger numbers with fourth
graders.
387
:Then the majority of the fourth graders
got the exact Bayesian answer.
388
:Of course, with conditional probabilism,
it would be totally lost.
389
:And also we have found that some, maybe
20% of the second graders find the
390
:Bayesian answer.
391
:The title of the paper is Our Children
Intuitive Basients.
392
:Yeah, it's in the show notes.
393
:And again, it's in the representation.
394
:It's a channel message in mathematics,
that representation of numbers matter.
395
:And if you don't believe it, just think
about doing a calculation or base rule
396
:with Roman numerals.
397
:Good luck.
398
:And that's well known in mathematics.
399
:For instance, the physicist...
400
:Feynman has made a point that
mathematically equally forms of a formula,
401
:or despite their mathematically
equivalent, they're not psychologically
402
:the same.
403
:Because, as I said, you can see new
directions, new guesses, new theories.
404
:In psychology, that is not always
realized.
405
:And what Feynman, Richard Feynman was
talking about would be called framing in
406
:psychology.
407
:And by many of my colleagues, it's
considered an error to pay attention to
408
:framing.
409
:It's not.
410
:It's an enabler for intelligent
decision-making.
411
:Yeah, this is fascinating.
412
:I really love that.
413
:And I really recommend your, your paper
that you that you're talking about.
414
:Do children have Bayesian intuitions?
415
:Because first, I really love the
experiment.
416
:I found that super, super interesting to
watch that.
417
:And also, yeah, as you were saying,
418
:in a way, the conclusion that we can draw
from that and basically how this could be
419
:integrated into how statistics education
is done, I think is extremely important.
420
:And actually, yeah, I wanted to ask you
about that.
421
:Basically, if you, what would be the main
thing you would change in the way
422
:statistical education is done?
423
:Well, so you're mainly based in Germany,
so I would ask in Germany, maybe just in
424
:general in Europe, since our countries are
pretty close on a lot of metrics.
425
:So I guess what you're saying for Germany
could also be applied for a lot of other
426
:European countries.
427
:it's actually starting to change.
428
:So some of my former post-docs are now
professors, and some are in education.
429
:And for instance, they have done
experiments in schools in Bavaria, where
430
:the textbooks have, in the 11th class,
have base rule.
431
:And they show trees, but with relative
frequencies.
432
:not natural frequencies.
433
:And I've run a study which basically
showed that when pupils learn in these
434
:textbooks base rules with relative
frequencies or conditional probabilities,
435
:and you test them later,
436
:90% can't do it anymore.
437
:They've done something like rote learning.
438
:Never understood it.
439
:And then, in class, teachers taught the
students natural frequencies they had
440
:never learned before.
441
:And then 90% could do it.
442
:Something they had never heard of.
443
:Thanks for watching!
444
:so my former students convinced the
Bavarian government with this study.
445
:And now natural frequencies and thus
understandable base is part of the mass
446
:curriculum in Bavaria.
447
:So that's a very concrete example where
one can help young persons to understand.
448
:And when they will be older and will be
doctors or have another profession where
449
:they need base, they will not be so
blocked and have to muddle through and not
450
:understand.
451
:And if there are patients, then they know
what to ask and how to find out what a
452
:positive HIV screening test really means
or a positive COVID test and what
453
:information one needs for that.
454
:So I think that statistical literacy is
one of the most important topics that
455
:should be taught in school.
456
:We still have an emphasis on the
mathematics on certainty, of certainty.
457
:So algebra, geometry, trigonometry,
beautiful systems.
458
:But what's most important for everyone in
later life is not geometry, it's
459
:statistical thinking.
460
:I mean in practical life.
461
:And we are missing to do that.
462
:The result is that...
463
:If you test people, including medical
professionals, or we have tested
464
:professional lawyers, with problems that
require Bayesian thinking, most are lost.
465
:And the level of statistical thinking
is...
466
:is often so low that you really can't
imagine it.
467
:Here's an example.
468
:Two years ago, the Royal Statistical
Society of London asked members of
469
:parliament whether they would be willing
to do a simple statistical test.
470
:And about 100 agreed.
471
:The first question was, if you throw a
fair coin twice, what's the chance that it
472
:will land twice on head?
473
:Now, if you think that every member of
parliament understands that there are four
474
:possibilities and two heads or two...
475
:So two heads are, that's one in fourth?
476
:No.
477
:About half understood and the others not.
478
:And the most wrong guess was it's still a
half.
479
:It's just an illustration of the level of
statistical thinking in our society.
480
:And I don't think if we would test German
politicians, we would do much better.
481
:And that's a, you might say, yeah, who
cares about coins?
482
:But look, there was COVID with all these
probabilities.
483
:There is investment.
484
:There are taxes.
485
:There are tons of numbers that need to be
understood.
486
:And if you have politicians that don't
even understand the most basic things,
487
:what can we expect?
488
:No, for sure.
489
:I completely agree.
490
:And these are topics we already tackled in
these podcasts, especially in episode 50,
491
:where I had David Spiegelhalter here on
the podcast.
492
:And we talked about these topics of
communication of uncertainty and all these
493
:very interesting topics, especially
education and how
494
:how to include all that in the education.
495
:So that these are very interesting and
important topics and I encourage people to
496
:listen to that episode, number 50 with
David Spiegelhauter.
497
:I will put it in the show notes.
498
:Yeah.
499
:I may add here that David and I have been
working together for many years.
500
:And he has been conducting the Wynton
Center for Evidence Communication or Risk
501
:Communication in Cambridge.
502
:And I'm still directing the Harding Center
for Risk Literacy.
503
:And both centers were funded by the same
person, David Harding, a London Investment
504
:Banker, who had insight that there's a
problem.
505
:But the rest of philanthropists don't
really seem to realize that it would be
506
:important to fund these centers.
507
:The Wyndham Center is now closed down.
508
:which is a great pity.
509
:And yeah.
510
:So there's very little funding for.
511
:So there's funding for research.
512
:So when I do the studies like this,
children, there's lots of funding for
513
:that.
514
:But the moment you apply what you learn
into the real world to help the society,
515
:funding stops.
516
:Except for...
517
:Philanthropes like David Harding.
518
:Mm-hmm.
519
:Any idea why that would be the case?
520
:They are the research agencies they think
they have not realized the problem that
521
:science is more than having publications.
522
:but that much of the science that we have
is actually useful.
523
:That's being realized in, if it's about
engineering, and it's about patent, yes,
524
:but that there are similar positive tools
that help people like natural frequencies
525
:to understand their world, and that you
can teach them, and then you need a few.
526
:guys who just go out and teach doctors,
lawyers or school children.
527
:That is not really in the mind of
politicians.
528
:Yeah, which is, which clearly is a shame,
right?
529
:Because you can see how important
probabilistic thinking is in a lot of, in
530
:a lot of fields.
531
:And, and, and especially in politics,
right?
532
:Even electoral forecasting, which is
something I've done a lot.
533
:Probabilistic thinking is absolutely,
absolutely of utmost importance.
534
:And yet, it's not there yet.
535
:and not a lot of interest in developing
this, at least in France, which is where I
536
:have done these experiments.
537
:That's always been puzzling to me,
actually.
538
:And even in sports, one of the recent
episodes I've done about soccer analytics
539
:with Maximilian Goebel, well,
540
:That was also an interesting conversation
about the fact that basically the methods
541
:are there to use the data more
efficiently, but a lot of European
542
:football clubs don't really use them for
some reason, which for me is still a
543
:mystery because that would help them make
better use of their finite resources and
544
:also be more competitive.
545
:So.
546
:Yeah, that's definitely something I'm
passionate to understand.
547
:So yeah, thanks a lot for doing all that
work.
548
:I'm here to try and help us understand all
that.
549
:everyone can help here.
550
:And for instance, most people are with the
doctors at some point, like COVID-19 or
551
:HIV tests or cancer screening.
552
:And everyone could ask the doctor, what's
the probability that I actually have the
553
:disease?
554
:or the virus, if it is positive.
555
:And then you likely will learn that your
doctor doesn't know that.
556
:Or excuse.
557
:Then you can help your doctor understand
that.
558
:And bring a natural frequency tree and
show them.
559
:I've done this with many doctors, but
quite a few.
560
:Over here, I said, I'm training doctors.
561
:I've trained more than 1,000, my own
researcher from the Harding Center, I've
562
:trained more than 5,000 extra.
563
:And the last time I was with my home
physician, I spent maybe 50 minutes with
564
:him.
565
:and 40 minutes explaining him on the
internet where he finds reliable
566
:information.
567
:The problem is not in the doctor's mind,
the problem is in the education, at the
568
:medical departments, where doctors learn
lots of things, but one thing they do not
569
:learn, statistical thinking.
570
:Mm-hmm.
571
:Yeah.
572
:with very few exceptions.
573
:And I'm curious, did you do some follow-up
studies on some courts of those doctors
574
:where you basically taught them those
tools, it seemed to work in the moment
575
:when they applied it, and then I'm curious
basically of the retention rate of these
576
:methods, basically is it something like,
oh yeah, when you force them in a way to
577
:use them, yeah, they see it's useful,
that's good.
578
:But then when you go away, they just don't
use them anymore.
579
:And they just refer to the previous way
they were doing things, which is of
580
:course, suboptimal.
581
:So yeah, I'm curious how that...
582
:continuing medley education, I have about
90 minutes and I teach them many things,
583
:not just natural frequencies.
584
:And when I teach them natural frequencies,
somewhere in the beginning, and I test
585
:them towards the end.
586
:So that's, yeah, a short time, a little
bit more than an hour.
587
:There is no way for me to find these
doctors again.
588
:But we have done follow-up studies up to
three months with students and teaching
589
:them how to translate conditional
probabilities in natural frequencies.
590
:And the interesting thing is that the
performance, which is after the training,
591
:around 90%, that means 90% of all tasks,
they get exactly right.
592
:After several months it stays at the same
level.
593
:Whereas in the control group where they
are taught conditional probability,
594
:exactly your problem is there.
595
:So they learn it not as well as natural
frequencies, but then a few days later it
596
:goes away and after three months they are
basically down with the story.
597
:Yeah.
598
:Some representations do not stick in the
minds.
599
:And frequency representations do, if they
are not relative frequencies.
600
:Yeah, this is definitely super
interesting.
601
:So basically to make it stick more, the
idea would be definitely use more natural
602
:frequencies.
603
:Is that what you were saying?
604
:Yes, and of course it doesn't hurt if you
continue thinking this way and do some
605
:exercise.
606
:Hmm, yeah.
607
:Yeah, yeah.
608
:I see.
609
:And something I'm also curious about and
that a lot of, a lot of beginners ask me a
610
:lot is what about priors, right?
611
:So I'm curious in your job, how did you
handle priors and the challenges regarding
612
:confirmation bias, persistence of...
613
:persistence of incorrect beliefs.
614
:So in a more general way, what I'm asking
is, how can individuals, particularly
615
:decision makers in fields like law or
medicine that you know very well, avoid
616
:the pitfalls associated with biased prior
beliefs and harnessing the power of
617
:patient reasoning?
618
:Yeah, so in the medical domain,
particularly in diagnostics, the priors
619
:are usually from, they're usually
frequencies and they are estimated by
620
:studies.
621
:There's always the possibility that a
doctor might adjust the frequency base
622
:rate a bit because he or she has some kind
of belief that
623
:this patient's main op-e exactly from that
group.
624
:But again, there's huge uncertainty about
priors.
625
:And also, one should not forget, there's
also uncertainty about likelihoods.
626
:Often in Beijing, the discussion centers
among priors.
627
:How do you know the likelihoods?
628
:So for instance, the, take the mammography
problem again, the probability that you
629
:test positive if you don't have cancer, so
which I in the example gave is 9%, which
630
:is roughly correct, but it varies.
631
:It depends on the age of the woman.
632
:It depends on quite a number of factors.
633
:And one should not forget that
634
:Also the likelihoods have to have some
kind of subjective element and judgment.
635
:And then there's a third more general
assumption, namely the assumption that all
636
:these terms, the likelihoods and the base
rates, which are from somewhere, maybe a
637
:study in Boston, would actually apply to a
study in Berlin.
638
:Mm-hmm.
639
:And I can name you a few more assumptions.
640
:For instance, that the world would be
stable, that nothing has happened.
641
:There's no different kind of cancer that
has different statistics.
642
:So one always has to assume a stable world
to do base.
643
:And one should be aware that it might not
be.
644
:And that's why I use the term statistical
thinking.
645
:Because you need to think about the
assumptions all the time and about the
646
:uncertainty in the assumptions.
647
:And also realize that often, particularly
if you have more complex problems, not
648
:just one test, but many, and many other
variables, you might, in these situations,
649
:where Bayes slowly gets intractable.
650
:Mm-hmm.
651
:You might think using a different
representation, like what we call a fast
652
:and frugal tree, that's a simple way.
653
:It's just like think about a natural
frequency tree, but it is an incomplete
654
:one, where you basically focus on the
important parts of the information and
655
:don't even try to estimate the rest in
order to avoid estimation error.
656
:And that's the key logic of heuristics.
657
:Under uncertainty, the big danger is that
you overfit.
658
:You overfit the data.
659
:You have wrongly assuming that the future
is like the past.
660
:And in order to avoid overfitting, as the
bias-variance dilemma shows in more
661
:detail, one needs to make things more
simple.
662
:Maybe not too simple, but more simple.
663
:and trying to estimate all conditional
probabilities may give you a great fit,
664
:but not good predictions.
665
:Yeah, so thanks a lot for this perfect
segue to my next question, because this is
666
:a recurring theme in your work and in your
research, simplicity.
667
:You often emphasize simplicity in
decision-making strategies.
668
:And so that was something I was wondering
about, because, well, I, of course, love
669
:Bayesian methods.
670
:They are extremely powerful.
671
:They are, most of the time,
672
:really intuitive to interpret, especially
the model parameters.
673
:But they are complex sometimes.
674
:And they appear even more complex than
they are to people who are unfamiliar with
675
:them, precisely because they are
unfamiliar with them.
676
:So anything you're unfamiliar with seems
extremely complex.
677
:So
678
:I'm wondering how we can bridge the gap
between the complexity of patient
679
:statistics, whether real or fantasized,
and the need for simplicity in practical
680
:decision-making tools, as you were talking
about, especially for professionals and
681
:the general public, because these are the
audiences we're talking about here.
682
:Now there are two ways.
683
:One is you stay within the Bayesian
framework and for instance avoid
684
:estimating conditional probabilities.
685
:And that would be what's called naive
Bayes.
686
:And naive Bayes can be amazingly good.
687
:It has also the advantage that is much
more easy to understand than regular
688
:Bayes.
689
:The second option is to leave the Bayesian
framework.
690
:and study how adaptive heuristics can give
you what base makes too complicated.
691
:And also there's too much overfitting.
692
:For instance, if we have studied
investment problems, so assume you have a
693
:sum of money and want to invest it in N
assets.
694
:How do you do it?
695
:And there are basic methods that tell you
how to weigh your money in each of these
696
:in assets.
697
:There is Markowitz Nobel Prize winning
method that's standards of statistics, the
698
:mean variance portfolio that tells you how
you should do that.
699
:But when Harry Markowitz made his own
investments for the time after his
700
:retirement...
701
:You might think he used his Nobel Prize
winning optimization method.
702
:No, he didn't.
703
:He used a simple heuristic that's called 1
over n, or divide equally, the same as a
704
:Bayesian equal prior.
705
:And a number of studies have asked how
good is 1 over n compared to the Nobel
706
:Prize?
707
:Winning Markowitz model and also modern
variants including Bayesian methods.
708
:The short answer is that 1 over n is
mostly as good as Markowitz and also
709
:better, and also the most modern
sophisticated models that use any kind of
710
:complexity cannot really beat it.
711
:The more interesting question is the
following.
712
:Can we identify in what situation
713
:A heuristic like 1 over n or any other of
the complicated models is ecologically
714
:rational.
715
:Because before we have talked about
averages.
716
:And you can see, so 1 over n has no free
parameter, very different from base.
717
:That means nothing needs to be estimated
from data.
718
:It actually doesn't need any data.
719
:Thus, in the statistical terms of bias and
variance, it may have a bias, and likely
720
:it has.
721
:So bias is the difference from the average
investment to the true situation, but it
722
:has no variance because it doesn't
estimate any parameters from data.
723
:And variance means it's the deviation.
724
:of individual estimates from different
samples around the average estimate.
725
:And since there is no estimate, there is
no variance.
726
:So Markowitz or Bayesian models, they
suffer from both errors.
727
:And the real question is whether the sum
of bias and variance of one method is
728
:larger than
729
:of the other one.
730
:And then ecologically rational it means,
let me illustrate this with the, with
731
:Markowitz versus Van der Weyne.
732
:So if you have more, if n is larger, then
you have more parameters to estimate
733
:because the covariances, they just
increase.
734
:That means more measurement error.
735
:So you can...
736
:derived from that, that in situations
where we have a large number of assets,
737
:then the complex methods will likely not
be as good.
738
:While 1 over n doesn't have more
estimation error, it has none anyhow.
739
:And then another thing is, if the true
distribution of
740
:the so-called optimal weights that you
only can know in the future, is highly
741
:skewed.
742
:Then 1 over n is not a good model for
that.
743
:But it's roughly equal, then that's the
case.
744
:So these are, and then sample size plays a
role for the estimation.
745
:So the more data you have, the Bayesian or
Markowitz model will profit, while it
746
:doesn't matter.
747
:for the 1 over n heuristic because it
doesn't even look at the data.
748
:So that's the kind of ecological
rationality thinking.
749
:And there are some estimates just to give
you some flesh into that.
750
:One study has asked, one study that found
that mostly in seven out of eight, I
751
:think, tests 1 over n made more money in
terms of Sharpe ratio and similar.
752
:criteria than the optimal Markowitz
portfolio and with 10 years of data.
753
:So they asked the question how many years
of data would one need so that the
754
:estimates get precise so that eventually
the complex model outperforms the simple
755
:heuristic.
756
:And that depends on the number of assets
you have.
757
:And if they are 50, for instance, then the
estimate is you need 500 years of stock
758
:data.
759
:So in the year 2500, we can turn to the
complex models, provided the same stocks
760
:are still around in the stock market in
the first place.
761
:That's a very different way to think about
a situation.
762
:It's the Herbert Simonian way, or don't
think about a method by itself, and don't
763
:ever believe that a method is rational in
every situation.
764
:But think about how this method matches
with the structure of environment.
765
:And that's a much more difficult question
to answer than just claiming that
766
:something is optimal.
767
:Yeah, I see.
768
:That's interesting.
769
:I love the very practical aspect of that.
770
:And also that, I mean, in a way that focus
on simplicity is something I found also
771
:very important in the way of basically
thinking about parsimony.
772
:Why make something more difficult when you
don't have to?
773
:And it's something that I always use also
in my teaching, where I teach how to build
774
:a model.
775
:Don't start with the hierarchical time
series model, but start with a really
776
:simple linear regression, which is just
one predictor, maybe.
777
:And don't make it hierarchical yet, even
though that makes sense.
778
:the problem at hand because from a very
practical standpoint if the model fails
779
:and it will at first if it's too complex
you will not know which part to take apart
780
:right and to and to make better so it's
just the parsimony makes it way easier to
781
:build the model and also to choose the
prior right just don't make your priors
782
:turn complicated find good enough priors
because you won't find
783
:Find good enough priors and then go with
that.
784
:I mean, the often use of the term optimal
is mostly misleading.
785
:Under uncertainty or interactability, you
cannot find the optimal solution and prove
786
:it.
787
:It's an illusion.
788
:And under uncertainty, so when you have to
make predictions, for instance, about the
789
:future and you don't know whether the
future is like the past,
790
:quite simple heuristics outperform highly
complex methods.
791
:An example is, remember when Google
engineers try to predict the flu with a
792
:system that's called Google Flu Trends.
793
:and it was a secret system and it started
with 45 variables, they were also secret,
794
:and the algorithm was secret.
795
:And it ran from 2008 till 2015.
796
:And at the very beginning in 2009 the
swine flu occurred.
797
:And out of season in the summer.
798
:And Google flew trends, so the big data
algorithm had learned that the flu is high
799
:in the winter and low in the summer.
800
:So it underestimated the flu-related
doctor visits, which was the criterion.
801
:And the Google engineers then tried to
revise the algorithm to make it better.
802
:And here are two choices.
803
:One is what I call the complexity
illusion, namely you have a complex
804
:algorithm and the high uncertainty, like
the flu is a virus that mutates very
805
:quickly, and it doesn't work.
806
:What do you do now?
807
:You make it more complex.
808
:And that's what the Google engineers did.
809
:So they used a revision with about 160
variables, also secret.
810
:and thought they would solve the problem,
but it didn't improve at all.
811
:The opposite reaction would have been...
812
:You have a complex and high uncertain
problem.
813
:You have a complex algorithm.
814
:It doesn't work.
815
:What do you do now?
816
:You make it simpler.
817
:Because you have too much estimation
error.
818
:The future isn't like the past.
819
:We have tested those published paper on a
very simple heuristic that just takes one
820
:data point.
821
:So remember that.
822
:Google Flu Trends estimated next week's or
this week's flu-related doctor visits.
823
:So the one data point algorithm is you
take the most recent data, it's usually
824
:one week or two weeks in the past, and
then make the simple prediction that's
825
:what it will be this or next week.
826
:That's a heuristic called the recency
heuristic, which is well documented in
827
:human thinking, is often mistaken as a
bias heuristic.
828
:And we showed it for the entire run of
Google Flu Trends for eight years.
829
:The simple heuristic outperformed Google
Flu Tense in all updates, about a total, I
830
:think, three updates.
831
:for every year and for each of the updates
and reduce the error by about half.
832
:You can intuitively see that.
833
:So a big data algorithm gets stuck like if
something unexpected happened like in the
834
:swine flu.
835
:The recency heuristic can quickly adapt to
the new situation and
836
:So that's another example showing that you
always should test a simple algorithm
837
:first.
838
:And you can learn from the human brain.
839
:So the heuristics we use are not what the
heuristics and bios people think, always
840
:second best.
841
:No.
842
:You need to see in a situation of high
uncertainty.
843
:Pick a right heuristic.
844
:A way to find it is to study what humans
do in these situations.
845
:I call this psychological AI.
846
:Yeah, I love that.
847
:Um, and actually that, so before closing
up the show that, um, sets us up nicely
848
:for one of my last questions, which is a
bit more, uh, formal thinking.
849
:Because so you, you've been talking about
AI and, and these decision-making science.
850
:So I'm wondering how you see the future of
decision science.
851
:And where do vision statistics fit into
this evolving landscape, especially
852
:considering the increasing availability of
data and computational power?
853
:And that may be related to your latest
book.
854
:Yeah.
855
:My latest book is about, it's called How
to Stay Smart in a Smart World, and it
856
:teaches one thing, a distinction between
stable worlds and unstable worlds.
857
:Stable worlds are like what the economist
Frank Knight called a situation of risk,
858
:where you can calculate the risk as
opposed to uncertainty.
859
:That's unstable worlds.
860
:If you have a stable world,
861
:That's the world of optimization
algorithms, at least if it's fractable.
862
:And here more data helps, because you can
fine-tune your parameters.
863
:If you have to deal with an unstable
world, and that's most of things are
864
:unstable, are not just viruses, but human
behavior.
865
:And complex algorithms typically do not
help in predicting human behavior.
866
:In my book I have a number of examples.
867
:And here you need to study smart adaptive
heuristics that help.
868
:And for instance, we are working with the
largest credit rating company in Germany.
869
:And they have...
870
:intransparent, secret, complex algorithms.
871
:That has caused an outcry in the public
because these are decisions that decide
872
:whether you are considered for, if you
want to rent a flat or not, and other
873
:things.
874
:And we have shown them that if they make
the algorithms simpler.
875
:then they actually get better and more
transparent.
876
:And that's an interesting combination.
877
:Here is one future about solving the
so-called XAI problem.
878
:First try a simple heuristic, that means a
simple algorithm, and see how good it is.
879
:And not just test competitively, a handful
of complex algorithms.
880
:Because the simple algorithm may be
881
:do as well or better than the complex
ones.
882
:And also they are transparent.
883
:And that means that doctors, for instance,
may accept an algorithm because they
884
:understand it.
885
:And a responsible doctor would not really
want to have a neural network diagnostic
886
:system that he or she doesn't understand.
887
:So the future of decision making would be,
if you want it in a few sentences, take
888
:uncertainty serious.
889
:and distinguish it from situations of
risk.
890
:We are not foreign, I hear this.
891
:And second, take heuristics seriously and
don't confuse them with viruses.
892
:And third...
893
:If you can, go out in the real world and
study decision making there.
894
:How firefighters like Gary Klein make
decisions, how chess masters make
895
:decisions, how scientists come up with
their theories.
896
:And you will find that standard decision
theory that's geared on small worlds of
897
:calculated risk will have little to tell
you about that.
898
:and then have the courage to study
empirically what experience people do, how
899
:to model this as heuristics and find out
their ecological rationality.
900
:That's what I see will be the future.
901
:Nice.
902
:Yeah, I find that super interesting in the
sense that it's also something I can see
903
:as an attractive feature of the patient
modeling framework from people coming to
904
:us for consulting or education, where the
fact that the models are clear on the
905
:assumptions.
906
:and the priors and the structure of the
model make them much more interpretable.
907
:And so way less black boxy than classic AI
models.
908
:And that's, yeah, definitely a trend we
see and it's also related to causal
909
:inference.
910
:People most of the time wanna know if X
influences Y and in what way, and if that
911
:is, you know, predictable way.
912
:And so for that causal inference,
913
:fits extremely well in the Bayesian
framework.
914
:So that's also something I'm really
curious about to see evolve in the coming
915
:years, especially with some new tools that
start to appear.
916
:Like I had Ben Vincent lately on the show
for episode 97, and we talked about causal
917
:pi and how to do causal inference in PyMC.
918
:And now we have the new do operator.
919
:in Pintsy, which helps you do that.
920
:So, yeah, I really love seeing all those
tools coming together to help people do
921
:more causal inference and also more state
of the art causal inference.
922
:And for the curious, we will do with
Benjamin Vincent a modeling webinar in the
923
:coming weeks, probably in September, where
he will demonstrate how to use the
924
:Dooperator in PIMC.
925
:So if you're curious about that, follow
the show.
926
:And if you are a patron of the show, you
will get early access to the recording.
927
:So if you want to support the show with...
928
:Cafe latte per month.
929
:Um, I, uh, I'm really, um, uh, thanking
you from the bottom of my heart.
930
:Um, well, Gert, um, I have so many other
questions, but I think, I think it's a
931
:good time to, to stop.
932
:Uh, I've already taken a lot of your time,
so I want to be mindful of that.
933
:Um, but before letting you go.
934
:I'm going to ask you the last two
questions I ask every guest at the end of
935
:the show.
936
:Number one, if you had unlimited time and
resources, which problem would you try to
937
:solve?
938
:I would try to solve the problem to
understanding the ecological rationality
939
:of strategies, particular heuristics.
940
:Hmm.
941
:That's a next.
942
:Yeah.
943
:You're the first one to answer that.
944
:And that's a very precise answer.
945
:I am absolutely impressed.
946
:And second question, if you could have
dinner with any great scientific mind,
947
:dead, alive, or fictional, who would it
be?
948
:Oh, I would love to have dinner with two
women.
949
:The first one is a pioneer of computers,
Ada Lovelace.
950
:And the second one is a woman of courage
and brain, Marie Curie.
951
:The only woman who got two Nobel Prizes.
952
:And Marie Curie said something very
interesting.
953
:Nothing in life.
954
:is to be feared.
955
:It is only to be understood.
956
:Now is the time to understand more so that
we may fear less." Kori said this when she
957
:discovered that she had cancer and was
soon to die.
958
:extremely inspiring.
959
:Yeah, thanks, Edgar.
960
:That's really inspiring.
961
:But having courage is something that's
very important for every researcher.
962
:And also having courage to look forward,
to dare, to find new avenues, rather than
963
:playing the game of the time.
964
:Well, on that note, I think, well, thank
you for coming on the show, Gert.
965
:That was an absolute pleasure.
966
:I'm really happy that we could have that
more, let's say epistemological discussion
967
:than we're used to on the podcast.
968
:I love doing that from time to time.
969
:Also filled with applications and
encourage people to take a look at the
970
:show notes.
971
:I put.
972
:your books over there, some of your
papers, a lot of resources for those who
973
:want to dig deeper.
974
:So thank you again, Gert, for taking the
time and being on this show.
975
:It was my pleasure.
976
:Bye bye.