This episode of Sports Visio Radio takes listeners to the iconic Fenway Park, weaving together legendary baseball history with groundbreaking sports vision science. From Ted Williams’ towering home run to Manny Ramirez’s unusual batting focus, the show unravels how elite athletes harness vision in ways far beyond ordinary “20/20 eyesight.”
At the center of the conversation is Dr. Daniel Laby, an ophthalmologist, sports vision specialist, and TEDx speaker at Fenway Park. With decades of experience working with elite athletes, Dr. Laby reveals why hitting a baseball—often called the hardest task in sports—is less about raw reflexes and more about prediction. He explains that a batter has only 100–150 milliseconds to process a pitch, less time than a blink of the eye, making vision and brain processing the ultimate differentiators.
The discussion highlights the limitations of traditional eye exams like the Snellen chart, which fail to replicate real-world, high-speed conditions. Dr. Laby’s innovative vision tests—smaller, lower-contrast targets flashed for fractions of a second—measure how athletes truly perform under game-like stress. His research has shown that MLB players typically see at 20/12, far sharper than average human vision, offering them a measurable edge.
Real-world stories bring this science to life: Stephen Drew’s postseason slump reversed after a simple contact lens prescription identified through advanced testing, and Manny Ramirez’s custom visual training drills, which helped him sharpen his pitch recognition and contributed to his World Series MVP performance. These examples underscore that vision isn’t just an accessory to athletic skill—it can be a game-changing factor when properly measured and trained.
Ultimately, the episode challenges how we think about vision in sports and life. True performance comes not just from clarity of sight but from optimizing the entire perception-to-action loop: seeing, processing, predicting, and executing. Whether at Fenway Park or in everyday challenges, the science of vision reveals there’s always more going on than meets the eye.
👉 Don’t forget to subscribe to Sports Vision Radio so you never miss an episode on the science of peak performance.
Welcome to the Deep Dive. Today we're digging into something pretty cool, the intersection of legendary sports moments and some really eye-opening science. Literally. Yeah. And we're starting somewhere truly special. Fenway Park, you know, home of the Boston Red Sox. Absolutely. You just think of Fenway and you picture it, right?
The green monster looming out there in left field Pesky pole down the right field line, and that famous red. See way out in the bleachers. What is it? 502 feet marks the longest home run ever hit there. Exactly. And it makes you think about the players who made that place. Legendary Ted Williams. Babe Ruth, way back when.
Giants of the game and Ted Williams hitting that monster shot. It kind of begs the question, doesn't it? It really does. Yeah. What made players like Williams so good? How could they possibly connect with a baseball like that? Well, that's what we're diving into and our guide today is, uh, some fascinating work from Dr. Daniel Laby
He's an ophthalmologist, a sports vision specialist, and get this, he actually gave a TED talk at Fenway Park discussing this very topic. Yep. Decades of experience working with elite athletes. Right. And his insights show that vision in sports, especially at the elite level, is way, way more than just, you know, having 20/20 eyesight.
So our mission here is to unpack that, explore the science, see how it actually impacts players, and maybe even change how you think about what good vision really means. Okay. Let's jump in. Alright, so Dr. Laby talks about hitting a baseball. What's the absolute core skill he points to? Is it just super fast reflexes?
It's actually something maybe less obvious. The number one thing for a batter prediction. Prediction, not react, prediction first, you have to figure out exactly where that little three-inch ball is gonna be in space in time. So you can hit it with a bat. That's only what, two and a quarter inches wide?
Precisely. And the timeframe involved. Well, that's where it gets kind of mind-blowing. Okay. Lay the physics on us. This part sounds wild. All right. So a 90 mile an hour pitch. It takes 400 milliseconds to get from the pitcher's hand to home plate. 400 milliseconds. It's like less than half a second already.
Seems impossible. Oh, but it gets worse. Much worse for the batter. You have to subtract the time it takes to actually swing the bat. Okay. How long does that take? About 150 milliseconds. So quick math, 400 minus one 50. Yeah. That leads 250 milliseconds. Right, but wait, there's more. Your brain needs time too.
Time to see the ball, process the information, send the signal to your muscles. The whole neural pathway thing, how much time does that eat up? Another hundred to 150 milliseconds. Seriously. So 250 minus, let's say 150. Leaves just a hundred milliseconds or maybe 150 milliseconds tops. That's the entire window.
A batter has to actually see the ball coming from 60 feet away. See it, figure out the spin, identify the pitch type, fast ball, curve ball, whatever, and start the decision to swing all in about a 10th of a second. That's insane. Dr. Laby puts it in perspective, right? With the whole blinking thing? Yeah, that's a great comparison.
A normal human eye blink takes about 300 milliseconds, 300. That's double or even triple the time. The batter has to process everything before committing to the swing. Exactly. It just hammers home why hitting a baseball consistently is considered one of the hardest things to do in any sport. Okay. So if it's not just about having.
Good eyes in the traditional sense. How does Dr. Laby define this sports vision? It's much broader. It's the entire system. How the visual info gets in through the eyes travels back to the visual part of the brain, okay. Then how it moves forward to the parts of the brain that make decisions. And then. Out to the motor system to actually execute the action, like swinging the bat.
So it's a whole chain reaction. Eyes, brain decision, action, the whole loop and optimizing that loop is key, not just in baseball. I mean, think about other sports. Wait, like a quarterback scanning the field, Dr. Laby mentioned Tom Brady, right? Having to see everything. Yeah. Or a point guard in basketball, a soccer player anticipating a pass.
It applies everywhere where you need fast visual processing and reaction. It really changes how you think about athletic ability. There's this whole hidden layer of processing and sometimes it leads to some weird-looking results. Like with Manny Ramirez. Ah, yes, the Manny picture. Dr. Laby shows this photo of Manny Ramirez at the exact moment he makes contact with the ball and where his eyes looking way out ahead, like towards the pitcher or even beyond, not at the ball at all, which seems totally wrong, right?
You're always told keep your eye on the ball. But here's the thing. Laby points out all the useful visual information Manny got it in that first hundred milliseconds or so of the pitch's flight. After that, it's too late to adjust anyway. Pretty much his brain had already processed where the ball would be initiated.
The swing, looking at the ball at impact, wouldn't actually help. He'd already done the work. He couldn't even blink if he wanted to. Right, because a blink takes too long. Exactly. It takes 300 milliseconds. The whole hitting event is faster than that. It's kind of funny, but it makes sense. Scientifically wild, and this elite level of vision.
search on this. Yeah. Back in:Kinder benchmark, right? 20/12 means that what an average person needs to be 12 feet away to see clearly what these baseball players can see just as clearly from 20 feet away. Exactly. They literally see detail better from further away than most people. It's a significant physical advantage, which makes you immediately question the standard eye test.
Like really old. Developed in:Totally. The chart is black letters on a white background, high contrast, right static. It doesn't move, and you basically have unlimited time to sit there and try to read the letters. Real life, especially sports is none of those things. Exactly. Real life involves split-second decisions. Things are moving fast, objects are small.
The contrast might be terrible, like seeing a white baseball against a bright sky or a red stitch pattern against a red background, maybe through dust, and often in less than perfect lighting, right? Leba uses that great analogy. Driving at dusk, it's foggy. You're in a strange place trying to read a street sign really quickly.
Yeah, that's a much better approximation of real world visual challenge than reading a static chart in a quiet room. Absolutely. So the smelling chart just not adequate for measuring the kind of functional vision these athletes need. So if the old chart is no good, what did Dr. Laby and his team come up with?
How do you test this kind of vision? They leverage technology. iPads computing power. They developed a new test. Okay. What does it look like instead of letters? The targets are circles with a gap, like a little open bar pointing either up, down, left or right. Think of the letter C rotated. Okay. Landalt C I think they're called, something like that.
But crucially for the athletes, these targets are much smaller than on a standard chart, and they aren't black on white. They're gray on white, much lower contrast, more realistic. And here's the kicker. They flash on the screen for only a hundred or 200 milliseconds. Ah. So matching the actual time window they have in a real game situation.
Exactly. It tests their ability to perceive detail quickly under time pressure with lower contrast. Then they use some pretty sophisticated math. Based on scoring methods like the SATs or ACTs to get a score that reflects their functional vision for hitting not just static acuity precisely, it measures how well their whole visual system performs under game-like stress.
nes. First one, Stephen Drew,:Stephen Drew is playing shortstop. Great glove, defensively solid, but hitting not so much. No. Oh man. He was in a terrible slump, like four for 40 overall in the postseason and in the World Series itself. He was one for 15. Oof. That's tough in the biggest games. Huge liability at the plate. So Dr. Laby tested him using this new advanced method and what did they find?
Something complex. Nope. Simple. He needed contact lenses. His vision wasn't quite sharp enough for hitting under pressure. Just needed contacts. So what happened? Dr. Laby got him the contacts the very next day. Now Drew felt weird wearing them in the field because he wasn't used to it. Okay. But he wanted to try them for batting.
So picture this, he runs into the tunnel in the dugout between innings, pops the contacts in, comes back out to hit in the middle of a World Series game. Yep. And in that game, after starting the series one for 15, he goes two for four. Two for four, including a home run. A really crucial home run that basically helped clinch the World Series title for the Red Sox that year.
Wow. From one for 15 to two for four with a homer just by putting in contacts identified by this new test, instant dramatic impact. It's pretty incredible shows how fine-tuned these athletes are and how even a small, correctly identified visual issue can make a huge difference. That's an amazing story.
It really drives home that these aren't just like minor tweaks. They can be game changers. Absolutely. And it highlights optimizing that whole visual cognitive function, not just assuming talent is everything. Okay, so that's Stephen Drew. What's the second story? Also, Manny Ramirez. Right. But a different issue.
Right. This was in:So this wasn't about needing glasses or contacts like Drew? No. This perfectly illustrates the difference between just seeing clearly static acuity and the dynamic visual motor skills, the reaction part of the equation. Many could see, but something felt off in his reaction or timing. That's how it seemed.
He wasn't feeling comfortable reacting to pitches. So Laby had to get creative with training, not just testing. Okay. What did he do? What kind of training? Tools? He started with something already out there. A sort of ring device with a wiffle ball attached. The ring rotates and you have to track the ball and catch it.
Sounds tricky. Laby says, most people find it really hard, but Manny. Manny being Manny, too easy. Of course. So round two, Laby made another ring this time with four different colored wiffle balls on it. Okay. Adding complexity. Still too easy for Manny. Huh? So Laby had to invent something new just for him?
Pretty much. Yeah. He describes it as inventing this third ring, and this one is really clever. What was on the third ring? Four actual baseballs. And they weren't just plain baseballs; they were painted with specific patterns. Patterns. What did the patterns represent? They were designed to mimic the look of different pitches coming at you.
The way the seam spin creates visual patterns. So one pattern might look like a fastball spin, another like a curveball, another a slider. Ah, based on how the seams and the spin affect air currents and how that looks visually. Exactly. Trying to replicate the visual cues, a hitter picks up in that first fraction of a second.
Okay, so he has this ring with four pattern baseballs. How did the training work? Laby would throw the ring towards Manny when the ring was about halfway there. Laby would yell out a pitch type, say fastball. Okay. Manny then had only that remaining distance that split second, to visually identify the baseball on the ring with the fastball pattern and then make the physical movement to intercept and catch that specific bull.
Exactly. It forced him to rapidly link visual identification of the pitch type with the correct motor response, just like hitting. That is brilliant. Did it work for Manny? Was this one challenging enough? This one, Manny found challenging. Effective. He apparently loved it. He started using this ring training drill before every single game for the rest of his career.
orld Series MVP that year. In:It really shows the power of moving beyond just acuity to training the whole dynamic visual motor system. Absolutely targeted training based on a deep understanding of how vision actually works in high-speed situations. It's about optimizing the whole perception-action cycle. These stories are fantastic.
They just make it so clear that sports vision isn't some vague concept. It's specific. Measurable, trainable, and incredibly important. Yeah. It goes way beyond just seeing clearly, it's reaction time, anticipation, concentration, how well your eyes and hands work together. Yeah. All that stuff. Whether you're trying to hit a 500 foot homer at Fenway, or you know, make a save in soccer or return a serve in tennis. The principles apply across the board. Dr. Laby's work, especially coming from that TED Talk right there at Fenway really opens up this whole dimension of elite performance. It's not just raw talent, it's science optimizing these subtle, crucial skills. So to wrap up, maybe a final thought for you to chew on.
Think back to that classic American poem, Casey at the bat. Mighty Casey. Striking out, right? Sad day in Mudville. But you have to wonder, right? If Casey had maybe worked with a sports vision specialist like Dr. Laby, got his dynamic vision tested, maybe did some ring drills, maybe there wouldn't have been sorrow in Mudville that day.
Maybe not. It just makes you think about all those unseen factors, the tiny details and perception and reaction that really do separate the good from the truly great, not just in sports, but probably in a lot of things we do. A fascinating thought to end on. There's always more going on than meets the eye, literally.
I hope you enjoyed the episode. Remember, take a look@sportsvision.nyc to get more information.